• No se han encontrado resultados

Barreras Psicológicas en Índices Bursátiles: Evidencia de Cuatro Países de Europa del Sur

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "Barreras Psicológicas en Índices Bursátiles: Evidencia de Cuatro Países de Europa del Sur"

Copied!
11
0
0

Texto completo

(1)

www.elsevier.es/cesjef

Cuadernos

de

economía

ARTICLE

Psychological

barriers

in

stock

market

indices:

Evidence

from

four

southern

European

countries

Júlio

Lobão

a,

,

Cristiano

Pereira

b

aUniversidadedoPorto---FaculdadedeEconomiadoPorto(UniversityofPorto---SchoolofEconomicsandManagement),CEPESE

---CentrodeEstudosdePopulac¸ão,EconomiaeSociedade,RuaDr.RobertoFrias,4200-464Porto,Portugal

bUniversidadedoPorto---FaculdadedeEconomiadoPorto(UniversityofPorto---SchoolofEconomicsandManagement),RuaDr.

RobertoFrias,4200-464Porto,Portugal

Received25May2016;accepted22October2016 Availableonline9December2016

JEL

CLASSIFICATION

G11; G12; G14; G15

KEYWORDS

Psychological barriers; M-values;

Stockmarketindices; Marketpsychology; Roundnumbers

Abstract In thispaper we examine,for the first time, themajor stockmarketindicesof Greece,Italy,Portugal,andSpainfor indicationofpsychologicalbarriers atroundnumbers. Uniformityinthetrailingdigitsoftheindiceswastested,andregressionandGARCHanalysis wasusedtoassessthedifferentialimpactofbeingaboveorbelowapossiblebarrier.Noevidence ofpsychologicalbarrierswasdetectedintheItalianstockmarket.Therewasweakevidence ofbarriersintheIberianstockmarkets, andastrong indicationofpsychologicalbarriersin theGreek stockmarket.Moreover, itis shownhere thattherelationship between riskand returntendstobeweakerattheproximityofroundnumbers,whichposesachallengetothe traditionalequilibriummodels.

© 2016 Asociaci´onCuadernos de Econom´ıa. Publishedby Elsevier Espa˜na, S.L.U. All rights reserved.

CÓDIGOSJEL

G11; G12; G14; G15

PALABRASCLAVE

Barreraspsicológicas; ValoresM;

Índicesbursátiles;

BarrerasPsicológicasenÍndicesBursátiles:EvidenciadeCuatroPaísesdeEuropadel Sur

Resumen Enesteartículoseexaminaporprimeravezlosprincipalesíndicesbursátilesde Grecia,Italia,PortugalyEspa˜naenbuscadeevidenciadebarreraspsicológicasennúmeros redondos.Hemosprobadolauniformidaddeladistribucióndedígitosdelosíndicesyhemos usadoregresionesyanálisisGARCHparaevaluarelimpactodiferencialdeestarporencimao pordebajodeunaposiblebarrera.Nosedetectóningunaevidenciadebarreraspsicológicasen elmercadobursátilitaliano.Existeevidenciapocosólidadebarrerasenlosmercadosibéricosy unafuerteindicacióndebarreraspsicológicasenelmercadogriego.Además,semuestraquela

Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddress:jlobao@fep.up.pt(J.Lobão). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cesjef.2016.10.005

(2)

Psicologiadel mercado;

Númerosredondos

relaciónentreelriesgoylarentabilidadtiendeasermásdébilenlaproximidaddelosnúmeros redondos,locualplanteaunretoalosmodelosdeequilibriotradicionales.

© 2016Asociaci´onCuadernos deEconom´ıa. Publicado porElsevier Espa˜na,S.L.U. Todoslos derechosreservados.

1.

Introduction

Market practitioners and journalists often refer to the existenceof psychologicalbarriersinstock markets.Many investorsbelievethatroundnumbersserveasbarriers,and that prices may resist crossing these barriers. Moreover, theuseoftechnicalanalysisisbasedontheassertionthat traderswill‘‘jumponthebandwagon’’of buying(selling) oncethepricebreaksup(down)througha‘‘psychologically important’’level thus suggesting that thecrossing of one of these barriers may push the prices up (down) more thanotherwisewarranted.Frequentlyusedphrasesbythe business press such as ‘‘support level’’ and ‘‘resistance level’’implythat,untilsuchtimeasanimportantbarrier is broken, increases and decreases in the prices may be restrained.

The impact of such kind of psychological barriers in investors’ decisions has been studied since the 1990s for a variety of asset classes, from exchange rates with De Grauwe and Decupere (1992) to stock options with Jang et al. (2015). The evidence of psychological barriers on stock market indices suggestssome significant impactsof thisphenomenonin thereturns andvariancesin different geographies and periods (e.g., Donaldson and Kim, 1993; KoedijkandStork,1994;Cyreeetal.,1999;Bahng,2003).

Thisarticleexaminestheexistenceofpsychological bar-riers at roundnumbers inthe majorstock market indices offourSouthernEuropean countries:Greece(FTSE/ATHEX Large Cap), Italy (FTSE MIB), Portugal (PSI 20) and Spain (IBEX35).Tothebestofourknowledge,noneofthese mar-kets hasever been analyzed withthis purpose. And their economic significance is not negligible: the four national stockmarketsaccountedin2012,inaggregate,forabouta thirdofeurozone’sGDPandalmostaquarterofeurozone’s totalstockmarketcapitalization(WorldBank,2014).

Theanchoringeffect,awell-knownbehavioralbiasfirstly identified by Tversky and Kahneman (1974), is the main explanation for the existence of psychological barriers in financialmarkets.Individuals,whenperformingan estima-tioninanambiguoussituation,tendtofixate(‘toanchor’) ona salient numbereven ifthat numberis irrelevant for theestimation.The anchoringonroundnumbers is impor-tantforitsgreatexplanatorypowerofsomeofthefeatures commonly associated to financial markets. It may help to understand, for example, the excessive price volatil-ity(Westerhoff,2003),themomentumeffect(Georgeand Hwang,2004),oreventheemergenceofspeculativebubbles (Shiller,2015).

Of course, behavioral biases are not the only reason whybarrierscouldexist.Forexample,thefactthatoption exercisepricesalsoareusuallyroundnumbers maybean additionalexplanationforthephenomenon.

The existence of psychological barrierscontradictsthe efficient market hypothesis as it points to some level of predictabilityinstockmarketsandthusmayleadto abnor-malrisk-adjustedreturns.Henceempiricalevidenceforthe existenceofpsychologicalbarriersisnotonlyofinterestto practitionerswhoarelookingforprofitablestrategiesbutit alsorepresentsa contributiontotheliteratureonmarket efficiencyandonmarketanomalies.

Ourmethodologycomprisesanumberofempiricaltests. We test for uniformity in the trailing digits of the stock indicesanduseregressionandGARCHanalysistoassessthe differentialimpactofbeingaboveorbelowapossible bar-rier.The resultsobtainedrevealsubstantialdifferencesin theincidence of psychological barriers onthe markets of thesample.IntheItalianstockmarket,itwasnotdetected anyevidenceofpsychological barriers.There isweak evi-denceofbarriersin theIberianstock markets.Lastly,the Greekstockmarketistheonewiththestrongestindications ofpsychological barriersnearbyroundnumbers.Moreover, weshowthattherelationshipbetweenriskandreturntends tobeweakerattheproximityofroundnumbers,especially inshorttimehorizons(uptofivedays).

Thisarticleisorganizedinasfollows.Section2reviews the empirical evidence regarding psychological barriers. Section3presentsthedataandmethodologiesusedinthis paper.Section 4 presentsthe empiricalresults. Section 5

offersconclusions.

2.

Previous

findings

Donaldson (1990a, 1990b) and De Grauwe and Decupere (1992)werethefirsttostudythe phenomenonof psycho-logicalbarriersandshowedthatroundnumbersareindeed ofspecialimportanceforinvestorsinthestockandinthe foreignexchangemarkets,respectively.Fromthenon, sev-eralother studiesfollowed,focusingnotonlyondifferent geographiesandperiods,butalsoondifferentassetclasses, suchasbonds,commoditiesandderivatives.

To date, stock indices have been the target of mostresearchconcerningpsychologicalbarriers.Donaldson (1990a,1990b)usedboth chi-squaredtestsandregression analysistotest for uniformity inthe trailing digitsof the DowJonesIndustrialAverage(DJIA),theFTSE-100,theTSE, andtheNikkei225.Hisfindingsrejecteduniformityforall buttheNikkeiindex.

(3)

effect’’. The same was not true to the less important Wilshire5000.

LeyandVarian (1994)alsostudied theDJIAconsidering a wider interval of time (1952---1993) and confirmed that therewere in fact fewer observations around 100-levels. In98.4%ofthetestedcases,uniformityinthetrailingdigits wasrejectedatthe95%significancelevel.Additionally,they emphasized the fact that non-uniform distribution of the finaldigitswasnotnecessarilysynonymofpricebarriersand foundnoevidenceofstockpricepredictabilityduetothese barriers.

Koedijk and Stork (1994) expanded the research to a numberofindices.Theauthorsstudiedtheexistenceof psy-chologicalbarriers onthe Brussels Stock Index(Belgium), ontheFAZGeneral (Germany),onthe Nikkei225 (Japan) andontheS&P500(U.S.)duringtheperiodJanuary1980to February1992,whiletheFTSE-100(U.K.)wasobservedfrom January1984toFebruary1992.Theydiscoveredsignificant indicationsof psychologicalbarriers’existenceontheFAZ General,theFTSE-100andtheS&P500,butweakindications ontheBrusselsIndex,andnonefortheNikkei225.AsinLey andVarian(1994),theyfailedtofindevidencesupporting thesignificanceof100-levelsinpredictingreturns.However, thismaybedueinparttothefactthattheydidnot disag-gregatethe effects of upward and downwardmovements throughbarriers.

DeCeusteretal.(1998)comparedthelastdigitsofDJIA, FTSE-100,ortheNikkei225withtheempiricaldistribution ofaMonteCarlosimulation.Theydidnotfindany indica-tionoftheexistenceofpsychologicalbarriersonthosethree indices.

Cyreeetal.(1999)showedthatthelasttwodigitsofthe DJIA,theS&P500,theFinancialTimesU.K.Actuaries (Lon-don)andtheDAXarenotequallydistributed.Pricesnextto barriersturnuplessfrequentlythanpricesinamoredistant position.The TSE 300,CAC 40,Hang SengandNikkei 225 exhibitsome significant evidence.They alsoanalyzed the distributionofthereturnswithregardtoexpectedreturns andvolatilityinamodifiedGARCHmodeltoconcludethat upwardmovementsthroughbarrierstendedtohavea con-sistentlypositiveimpactontheconditionalmeanreturnand alsothatconditional variancetended tobe higherin pre-crossingsubperiodsandlowerinpost-crossingsubperiods.

More recently, Bahng (2003) applied the methodology ofDonaldsonandKim (1993)toanalyzesevenmajorAsian indicesincludingSouthKorea,Taiwan,HongKong,Thailand, Malaysia,Singapore,andIndonesiabetween1990and1999. TheiranalysisshowedthattheTaiwaneseindexdidpossess pricebarrier effects andthatthe priceleveldistributions oftheTaiwanese, Indonesian,andHongKongindiceswere explainedbyquadraticfunctions.

DorfleitnerandKlein(2009)focusedontheDAX30,the CAC 40, the FTSE-50 and the Euro-zone-related DJEURO STOXX50fordifferentperiodsuntil2003.Theyfoundfragile tracesof psychologicalbarriers inall indicesat the 1000-level.Therewerealsoindicationsofbarriersatthe100-level exceptintheCACindex.

Finally,Jangetal.(2015)consideredthe15-mininterval historicalrecordsof the S&P 500Indexfrom July 8,2011 toJanuary19,2012,andfoundsignificantevidenceof psy-chologicalbarriersateach100level.Moreover,theauthors suggestedanewmodelthatincorporatestheimpactofthose

barriersandcomparedthemodel’sempiricalperformance withrespecttotheBlack---Scholesandconstantelasticityof variancemodels.

Otherstudiesconcerningpsychologicalbarriersinstock markets arealso relatedtoour analysis.It is thecase of thosearticlesthataddressthepresenceofbarriersin indi-vidualstockpricessuchasCaietal.(2007)andDorfleitner andKlein(2009).

Caietal.(2007)assessedtheexistenceofpsychological barriersina totalof1050A-sharesand100 B-sharesfrom boththeShanghaiStockExchangeandtheShenzhenStock Exchange duringJune 2002.Arangeof measuresforprice resistanceshowedthedigits0and5tobesignificant resis-tancepointsintheA-sharemarket.Noresistancepointwas foundintheShanghaiB-sharemarket,althoughdigit0has hadthehighestlevelofresistancecomparedtoothers.

DorfleitnerandKlein(2009)analyzedeightmajorstocks from theGerman DAX 30 over the periodMay 1996---June 2003. The prices were examined withrespect to the fre-quencywithwhichtheyliedwithina certainbandaround thebarrier.Inaddition,theystudiedbarrier’sinfluenceon intradayvariancesandthedailytradingvolume.Overall,the authorswerenotabletoidentifyasystematicandconsistent patternatbarriers.

Different studies concluded that price barriers or at least significant deviations from uniformity also exist in other asset classes such as exchange rates (De Grauwe and Decupere, 1992), bonds (Burke, 2001), commodities (AggarwalandLucey,2007)andderivatives(Schwartzetal., 2004;ChenandTai,2011;Jangetal.,2015;Dowlingetal., 2016). Overall,evidence of pricebarriersin variousasset classesseemstobefairlyrobust.

3.

Data

and

methodology

3.1. Data

The examination window for each of the stock market indicesunderstudyispresented inTable1.Startingdates aredifferentsinceweusedthedatapertainingeachindex sinceitsinception.Allthedatawereretrievedfrom Thom-son Reuters Datastream. Summary statistics on the stock pricesarepresented inTable2 whereit canbeseen that the measures of skewness and,especially, kurtosis are in generalinconsistentwithnormality.

3.2. Methodology

3.2.1. Definitionofbarriers

(4)

Table1 Datausedinthestudy.

Country Stockindex Startingdate Endingdate

Greece FTSE/ATHEXLargeCap September23rd,1997

December31, 2013

Italy FTSEMIB December31st,1997

Portugal PSI20 December31st,1992

Spain IBEX35 January5th,1987

Table2 Summarystatisticsonstockpricesdataseries.

Country N Returnseries Levelseries

Mean Std.dev. Skewness Kurtosis Maximum Minimum

Greece 4245 0.000002 0.001784 0.98993 37.9344 3301.69 169.88

Italy 4174 0.000000 0.000032 0.11526 11.8689 50,108.56 12,362.51

Portugal 5478 0.000002 0.000071 0.16431 13.5654 14,822.59 2917.56

Spain 7041 0.000003 0.000126 −0.05759 22.3159 15,945.70 1873.58

usedintheliteratureaboutpsychologicalbarriers.Formally, wemayconsiderfourpossiblebarrierbands:

M100:Barrierlevell=3(1000s) 980-20;950-50 M10:Barrierlevell=2(100s) 98-02;95-05 M1:Barrierlevell=1(10s) 9.8-0.2;9.5-0.5 M0.1:Barrierlevell=0(1s). 0.98-0.02;0.95-0.05

3.2.2. M-values

M-valuesrefertothelastdigitsintheintegerportionofthe indicesunderanalysis.InitiallyusedbyDonaldsonandKim (1993),M-valuesconsideredpotentialbarriersatthelevels

...,300,400,...,3400,3500,i.e.at:

k×100, k=1,2,... (1)

Later,DeCeusteretal.(1998)claimedthatthisdefinition wastoonarrowbecausetheserieswasnotmultiplicatively regenerative, resulting, for instance, on 3400 being con-sideredabarrier,whereas340wouldnot.Additionally,the authorsclaimedthat,asdefinedbyEq.(1),thegapbetween barrierswouldtendtozeroasthepriceseriesincreased, dis-ruptingtheintuitiveappealofapsychologicalbarrier.Thus, one shouldalso considerthe possibilityof barriersat the levels...,10,20,...,100,200,...,1000,2000,...,i.e.at:

k×10l, k=1,2,...,9; l=...,−1,0,1,...; (2)

and,ontheotherhand,at thelevels...,10,11,...,100, 110,...,1000,1100,...,i.e.at:

k×10l, k=10,11,...,99; l=...,−1,0,1,...; (3)

M-valueswouldthenbedefinedaccordingtothese bar-riers.ForbarriersatthelevelsdefinedinEq.(1),M-values wouldbethepairofdigitsprecedingthedecimalpoint:

Ma

t =[Pt]mod100; (4)

wherePtistheintegerpartofPtandmod100referstothe reductionmodulo100.Forbarriersatthelevelsdefinedby Eqs.(2)and(3),theM-valueswouldbedefinedrespectively

asthesecondandthirdandthethirdandfourthsignificant digits.Formally,

Mb

t=[100×10(logPt)mod1]mod100; (5)

Mc

t=[100×10(logPt)mod1]mod100; (6)

where logarithms are to base 10. In practical terms, if

Pt=1234.56, then Mat =34. At this level, barriers should appearwhenMat=100.Additionally,Mbt=23andMct=12.

3.2.3. Uniformitytest

Having computed the M-values,the next step consists of examining the uniformity of their distribution. Following

Aggarwal and Lucey (2007), this will be done through a Kolmogrov---SmirnovZ-statistic test. Thuswe willbe test-ingH0:uniformityoftheM-valuesdistributionagainstH1: non-uniformityoftheM-valuesdistribution.

Itisimportanttoemphasizethattherejectionof unifor-mitymightsuggesttheexistenceofsignificantpsychological barriersbutitisnotinitselfsufficienttoprovetheexistence ofpsychologicalbarriers.LeyandVarian(1994)showedthat thelastdigitsoftheDowJonesIndustrialAveragewerein factnotuniformlydistributedandevenappearedtoexhibit certainpatterns, but the returns conditional on the digit realizationwerestillsignificantlyrandom.Additionally, De Ceuster et al. (1998) noted that as a series grows with-outlimitandtheintervalsbetweenbarriersbecomewider, thetheoreticaldistributionofdigitsandtherespective fre-quencyofoccurrencearenolongeruniform.

3.2.4. Barriertests

(5)

Wewillusetwotypesofbarriertests:thebarrierproximity testandthebarrierhumptest.

3.2.4.1. Barrier proximity test. This test examines the

frequencyofobservations,f(M),nearpotentialbarriersand willbeperformedaccordingtoEq.(7).

f(M)=˛+ˇD+ε (7)

The dummyvariable willtake the valueof unity when theindexisatthesupposedbarrierandzeroelsewhere.As itwasmentionedinSection 3.2.1,thisbarrierwillnotbe strictlyconsidered asan exactnumberbutalsoasa num-berofdifferentspecificintervals,namelywithanabsolute lengthof2% and5%of thecorrespondingpowerof tenas barriers.The nullhypothesis ofnobarrierswillthusimply thatˇequalszero,whileˇisexpectedtobenegativeand significantin thepresenceof barriersasaresultoflower frequencyofM-valuesattheselevels.

3.2.4.2. Barrierhump test. The second barrier test will

examine not just the tails of frequency distribution near thepotential barriers, but the entire shapeof the distri-bution.Itisthus necessarytodefinethealternativeshape thatthedistributionshouldhave inthepresenceof barri-ers(Donaldson andKim,1993;AggarwalandLucey,2007).

Bertolaand Caballero (1992), who analyzed the behavior ofexchangeratesinthepresenceoftargetzonesimposed byforward-lookingagents,suggestthatahump-shapeisan appropriatealternativeforthedistributionofobservations. The test toexamine thispossibilitywillfollowEq.(8), in which thefrequency of observationof each M-value is regressedontheM-valueitselfandonitssquare.

f(M)=˛+˚M+M2+ (8)

Underthenullhypothesisofnobarriersisexpectedto bezero,whereasthepresenceofbarriersshouldresultin beingnegativeandsignificant.

3.2.5. Conditionaleffecttests

TherejectionofuniformityontheobservationsofM-values isnotsufficienttoprovetheexistenceofpsychological bar-riers(Leyand Varian, 1994).Therefore,it is necessary to analyze the dynamics of the returns series around these barriers,namely regardingmean andvariance in orderto examine the differential effect on returns due to prices beingnearabarrier,andwhetherthesebarrierswerebeing approached on an upward or on a downward movement (Cyreeetal.,1999;AggarwalandLucey,2007).

Accordingly,wewillthusdefinefourregimesaround bar-riers:BDfor thefivedaysbeforepricesreachingabarrier onadownwardmovement,ADforthefivedaysafterprices crossinga barrier ona downwardmovement, andBU and AUfor the five days respectively before and after prices breachingabarrieronanupwardmovement.Thesedummy variableswilltakethevalueofunityforthedaysnotedand zerootherwise.Toexaminetherobustnessoftheresultsto theassumption that the regimeshould last for five days, wewillalsoconsiderawindowof10daysasinCyreeetal. (1999).Intheabsenceofbarriers,weexpectthecoefficients ontheindicatorvariablesinthemeanequationtobe non-significantlydifferentfromzero.

Rt=ˇ1+ˇ2BDt+ˇ3ADt+ˇ4BUt+ˇ5AUt+εt (9)

Following Aggarwal andLucey (2007), we started with an OLS estimation of Eq. (9) but heteroscedasticity and autocorrelation wereclearly present acrossour database. Therefore, the full analysis of the effects in the proxim-ityofbarriersrequiredustoapplytheformertest alsoto the variances. Eq.(10)representsthisapproachassuming autocorrelationsimilartooneasinCyreeetal.(1999)and

Aggarwal and Lucey (2007). Besides the abovementioned dummy variables it includes a moving average parameter andaGARCHparameter.

εt=N(0,Vt)

Vt=˛1+˛2BDt+˛3ADt+˛4BUt+˛5AUt+˛6Vt−1

+˛7ε2

t−1+t

(10)

Thefourpossiblehypothesestobetestedarethe follow-ing:

H1. Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalmeanreturn beforeandafteradownwardcrossingofabarrier.

H2. Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalmeanreturn beforeandafteranupwardcrossingofabarrier.

H3. Thereisnodifferenceinconditionalvariancebefore andafteradownwardcrossingofabarrier.

H4. There is no difference in the conditional variance beforeandafteranupwardcrossingofabarrier.

4.

Empirical

findings

4.1. Uniformitytest

Table3providestheresultsofauniformitytestconcerning thedistributionofdigitsfor thefourstock marketindices under scrutiny.Overall, there is robustevidence that the

M-values do not follow a uniform distribution in three of the fourstock markets. Uniformity is clearly rejectedfor PortugalandSpainat allsignificancelevels.Inthecase of theItalianmarket,uniformityisrejectedat5%onlyinthe three lowerbarrier levels. The rejection of uniformity of thetrailingdigitsisnotsostrongintheGreekmarket:ata significancelevel of5%,uniformityisrejectedonlyat the highestbarrierlevel.

4.2. Barriertests

4.2.1. Barrierproximitytest

Results for the barrier proximity tests are shown in

Tables4---6fortheintervalsmentionedinSections3.2.1and 3.2.4. As referredabove,in the presenceof a barrierwe would expectˇtobenegativeand significant,implying a lowerfrequencyofM-valuesatthesepoints.Consideringa barrierintheexactzeromodulopoint,evidenceinTable4

(6)

Table3 Kolmogorov---Smirnovtestforuniformityofdigits.

Country Statistic M0.1(l=0) M1(l=1) M10(l=2) M100(l=3)

Greece Kolmogorov(D)---Statisticvalue(adjusted) 1.278

* 1.201 1.093 5.457***

p-Value 0.076 0.112 0.183 0.000

Italy Kolmogorov(D)---Statisticvalue(adjusted) 1.721

*** 1.637*** 1.560** 0.860

p-Value 0.005 0.009 0.015 0.450

Portugal Kolmogorov(D)---Statisticvalue(adjusted) 2.322

*** 2.529*** 1.629*** 2.067***

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.010 0.000

Spain Kolmogorov(D)---Statisticvalue(adjusted) 6.714

*** 2.583*** 1.990*** 1.781***

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.004

ThetableshowstheresultsofaKolmogorov---Smirnovtestforuniformity.Eachtestwasperformedforthedailyclosingpricesofeach stockindex.Dstandsforthevalueoftheteststatisticwhilep-valuegivesthemarginalsignificanceofthisstatistic.H0:uniformityin thedistributionofdigits,H1:non-uniformityinthedistributionofdigits.

* Significanceat10%level.

** Significanceat5%level.

*** Significanceat1%level.

Table4 Barrierproximitytest:strictbarrier.

Countries M0.1(l=0) M1(l=1) M10(l=2) M100(l=3) ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square Greece −0.002 0.487 0.005 −0.003 0.433 0.006 −0.010*** 0.027 0.049 0.012 0.129 0.002

Italy −0.005 0.468 0.005 0.000 0.991 0.000 −0.001 0.848 0.000 −0.005 0.500 0.000

Portugal −0.010* 0.064 0.035 −0.005 0.198 0.017 −0.007 0.287 0.012 −0.006 0.232 0.001

Spain −0.010 0.540 0.004 −0.009** 0.044 0.041 0.008 0.152 0.021 0.001 0.781 0.000

Thetableshowstheresultsofaregressionf(M)=˛+ˇD+ε,wheref(M)standsforthefrequencyofappearanceoftheM-values,Disa dummyvariablethattakesthevalueofunitywhenM=00and0otherwise.RefertoSection3.2.4fordetails.

* Significanceat10%level.

** Significanceat5%level.

*** Significanceat1%level.

Table5 Barrierproximitytest:98-02barrier.

Countries M0.1(l=0) M1(l=1) M10(l=2) M100(l=3)

ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square Greece 0.001 0.352 0.009 −0.001 0.535 0.004 0.000 0.920 0.000 −0.005*** 0.000 0.013

Italy 0.002 0.569 0.003 0.003 0.260 0.013 0.000 0.967 0.000 ---0.001 0.516 0.000

Portugal 0.001 0.570 0.003 0.001 0.421 0.007 −0.001 0.665 0.002 0.191*** 0.001 0.002

Spain 0.006 0.395 0.007 0.003 0.195 0.017 0.000 0.916 0.000 0.000 0.943 0.000

Thetableshowstheresultsofaregressionf(M)=˛+ˇD+ε,wheref(M)standsforthefrequencyofappearanceoftheM-values,Disa dummyvariablethattakesthevalueofunitywhenM=valueisinthe98-02intervaland0otherwise.RefertoSection3.2.4fordetails.

*** Significanceatthe1%level.

Table6 Barrierproximitytest:95-05barrier.

Countries M0.1(l=0) M1(l=1) M10(l=2) M100(l=3)

ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square ˇ p-Value R-square Greece 0.001 0.441 0.006 −0.001 0.578 0.003 −0.001 0.492 0.005 −0.004*** 0.000 0.021

Italy 0.001 0.625 0.002 0.000 0.790 0.001 −0.002 0.266 0.013 −0.001 0.146 0.002

Portugal 0.000 0.842 0.000 0.000 0.751 0.001 0.000 0.912 0.000 0.002*** 0.002 0.009

Spain −0.001 0.858 0.000 0.001 0.649 0.002 0.000 0.978 0.000 −0.001 0.256 0.001

Thetableshowstheresultsofaregressionf(M)=˛+ˇD+ε,wheref(M)standsforthefrequencyofappearanceoftheM-values,Disa dummyvariablethattakesthevalueofunitywhenM=valueisinthe95-05intervaland0otherwise.RefertoSection3.2.4fordetails.

(7)

Table7 Barrierhumptest.

Countries M0.1(l=0) M1(l=1) M10(l=2) M100(l=3)

p-Value R-square p-Value R-square p-Value R-square p-Value R-square Greece 2.20E−07 0.616 0.003 −1.78E−07 0.746 0.002 −2.54E−07 0.683 0.003 −1.00E−08*** 0.005 0.017

Italy 3.20E−07 0.705 0.001 2.30E−07 0.739 0.005 −3.60E−07 0.569 0.009 −4.00E−09 0.192 0.002 Portugal 5.40E−08 0.941 0.004 1.50E−07 0.758 0.015 1.60E−07 0.850 0.001 9.00E−09*** 0.000 0.021

Spain −4.70E−08 0.983 0.007 3.00E−07 0.612 0.011 −8.00E−08 0.916 0.003 0.00E+00 0.924 0.000

Thetableshowstheresultsofaregressionf(M)=˛+ϕM+M2+,wheref(M),thefrequencyofappearanceofeachM-values,isregressed onM,theM-valueitself,andM2,itssquare.

*** Significanceatthe1%level.

Table5).Consideringthe95-05interval,Table6showsthat thenobarrierhypothesisisagainrejectedonlyforGreece inthesamecircumstances. Alltheother seriesareeither notsignificantorˇisnotnegative.Inanycase,thereareno evidenceofpsychologicalbarriersaroundroundnumbersin theItalianstockmarket.

Overall,evidenceisscatteredasthereisnoclearpattern regardlessoftheintervalweconsiderforthebarrier with thepossibleexceptionofa1000-levelbarrierintheGreek stockmarket.R-squaresaresignificantlylow,whichisinline withpreviousstudies.

4.2.2. Barrierhumptest

Table7shows theresultsfor thebarrier humptest,which ismeanttotesttheentireshapeofthedistributionofM -values.Assumingitshouldfollowahump-shapedistribution, wethusexpectedtobenegativeandsignificantinthe pres-enceofbarriers.Theresultsofthebarrierhumptestconfirm theevidencepresentedpreviouslywiththebarrier proxim-itytests. Again, it is the Greek stock market that stands out:itistheonlymarketthatexhibitsapersistentbarrier, namelyatthe1000-levelbarrier,atastatisticallysignificant levelof1%.

Overall,fromtheresultspresentedsofaritispossibleto discernsubstantialdifferencesin theincidenceof psycho-logicalbarriersonthemarketsofthesample.IntheItalian stockmarket,itwasnotdetectedanyevidenceof psycho-logical barriers.In the case of theIberian stock markets, thereisweakevidenceofbarriersatlowlevels(1-level bar-rierand10-levelbarrier).Lastly,theGreekstockmarketis theonewiththestrongestindicationsofpsychological bar-riersnearbyroundnumbers.Ahighlysignificantbarrierwas detectedatthehighestlevel(1000-levelbarrier).

4.2.3. Conditionaleffectstest

Assuming the existence of psychological barriers, we expected the dynamics of return series to be different aroundthesepoints.Infact,resultsinTable8providesome interestingevidenceofmeaneffectsaroundbarriersasitis observed,ononehand,thatstockmarketreturnsinallfour marketstendtobesignificantlyhigherwhenabarrieristo becrossedinanupwardmovement.Ontheotherhand,the coefficientsof BDand AD arenegativeand significant for allindiceswhichmeansthatstockmarketreturntendtobe significantlylowerintheproximity ofabarrier whenthat barrieris tobecrossedona downwardmovement. These patternofconditionaleffectsissimilartotheoneobtained byCyreeetal.(1999).

Inordertoprovidesomeevidenceof therobustnessof these findings, we estimate the same model with a time windowof10days.TheresultsarepresentedinTable9and areverysimilar,bothinmagnitudeandsignificance,tothose presentedinTable8.

Table 10 contains results for the conditional variance equation. The constant is positive and significant for all indices. All coefficients of the lagged squared residuals arepositiveandsignificantat the1%levelpointingoutto an increasein conditionalvariance coincidentwithhigher residuals from the previous period. The GARCH term in theconditionalvarianceispositiveandsignificant, suggest-ingsignificant GARCHeffects aroundbarriers.The GARCH termcorrespondingtotheSpanishmarketis closertoone whichindicatesahigherlevelofvolatilitypersistence.The volatility persistence in Spain, Italy and Portugal is quite higherthan Cyreeetal.(1999) have foundin other Euro-pean stock markets. The variance effects areparticularly evidentbeforeanupwardmovementthroughabarrier:the coefficient ofBU in thevarianceequationis negativeand statisticallysignificantinallthemarkets understudy.This indicatesthatthemarketstendtocalmbeforehavingrisen through a barrier. This is in sharpcontradiction with the resultsobtainedbyCyreeetal.(1999)accordingtowhich,in mostcases,marketstendtobemorevolatilebefore cross-ing abarrier in an upwardmovement. Inthe pre-crossing periodbutinthecaseofadownwardmovement,theresults areheterogeneous:ItalyandPortugalhave non-significant resultswhereasthecoefficientscorrespondingtotheGreek marketandtotheSpanishmarketaresignificantlypositive andsignificantlynegative,respectively.

Theresultsinthepost-crossingperiodarealsosomewhat heterogeneous. The Spanish stock market is the only one that exhibitsa statisticallysignificant resultinthe coeffi-cientofAU.ThevolatilityoftheallbuttheGreekmarket significantlyincreasesaftercrossingabarrierinadownward movement,aresultthatisinlinewithCyreeetal.(1999).

(8)

Table8 GARCHanalysis:meanequation(5days).

5Days ˇ1 BD AD BU AU

Greece Coefficient 1.93E−05

*** 2.50E04*** 2.00E04*** 2.31E04*** 1.18E04***

p-Value 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.003

Italy Coefficient 1.75E−07 −4.88E−06

*** 5.22E06*** 5.12E06*** 4.25E06***

p-Value 0.568 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000

Portugal Coefficient 2.77E−06

*** 2.69E05*** 1.81E05*** 1.95E05*** 2.18E05***

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000

Spain Coefficient 3.60E−06

*** 2.67E05*** 2.29E05*** 2.11E05*** 1.97E05***

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000

ThetableshowstheresultsofthemeanequationofaGARCHestimationoftheformRt=ˇ1+ˇ2BD+ˇ3AD+ˇ4BU+ˇ5AU+εttN(0,Vt);

Vt=˛1+˛2BD+˛3AD+˛4BU+˛5AU+˛6Vt−1+˛7ε2t−1+t.BD,AD,BUandAUaredummyvariables.BDtakesthevalue1inthe5

daysbeforecrossingabarrieronadownwardmovementandzerootherwise,whereasADisforthe5daysafterthesameevent.BUis forthe5daysbeforecrossingabarrierfrombelow,whileAUis1inthe5daysafterthesameupwardcrossing.Vt−1referstothemoving

averageparameterandε2

t−1standsfortheGARCHparameter. *** Significanceatthe1%level.

Table9 GARCHanalysis:meanequation(10days).

10Days ˇ1 BD AD BU AU

Greece Coefficient 1.60E−05

*** 1.70E04*** 1.50E04*** 1.56E04*** 1.15E04***

p-Value 0.008 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.002

Italy Coefficient 8.05E−08 −3.06E−06

*** 3.41E06*** 3.65E06*** 2.58E06***

p-Value 0.858 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000

Portugal Coefficient 2.66E−06

*** 1.89E05*** 1.44E05*** 1.67E05*** 1.35E05***

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000

Spain Coefficient 6.13E−06

*** 2.49E05*** 1.65E05*** 1.23E05*** 2.15E05***

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000

ThetableshowstheresultsofthemeanequationofaGARCHestimationoftheformRt=ˇ1+ˇ2BD+ˇ3AD+ˇ4BU+ˇ5AU+εttN(0,Vt);

Vt=˛1+˛2BD+˛3AD+˛4BU+˛5AU+˛6Vt−1+˛7ε2t−1+t.BD,AD,BUandAUaredummyvariables.BDtakesthevalue1inthe10

daysbeforecrossingabarrieronadownwardmovementandzerootherwise,whereasADisforthe10daysafterthesameevent.BU isforthe10daysbeforecrossingabarrierfrombelow,whileAUis1inthe10daysafterthesameupwardcrossing.Vt−1referstothe

movingaverageparameterandε2t−1standsfortheGARCHparameter.

*** Significanceatthe1%level.

Table10 GARCHanalysis:varianceequation(5days).

5Days ˛1 ε2t1 Vt−1 BD AD BU AU

Greece Coefficient 4.00E−09

*** 0.2051*** 0.7983*** 4.30E08*** 1.40E08** 2.00E08*** 8.00E09

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.038 0.000 0.219

Italy Coefficient 2.60E−12

*** 0.1073*** 0.8948*** 2.60E12 9.50E12*** 7.80E12*** 3.00E13

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.222 0.000 0.000 0.877

Portugal Coefficient 3.64E−11

*** 0.125*** 0.8729*** 4.00E11 1.05E10** 1.10E10*** 3.82E11

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.403 0.036 0.000 0.27

Spain Coefficient 1.90E−11

*** 0.0786*** 0.9263*** 1.00E10** 2.00E10*** 5.00E11* 8.70E11**

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.026 0.000 0.081 0.020

The table shows the results of the variance equation of a GARCH estimation of the form Rt=ˇ1+ˇ2BD+ˇ3AD+ˇ4BU+ˇ5AU+εt;

εtN(0,Vt);Vt=˛1+˛2BD+˛3AD+˛4BU+˛5AU+˛6Vt−1+˛7ε2t−1+t.BD,AD,BUandAUaredummyvariables.BDtakesthevalue 1inthe5daysbeforecrossingabarrieronadownwardmovementandzerootherwise,whereasADisforthe5daysafterthesame event.BUisforthe5daysbeforecrossingabarrierfrombelow,whileAUis1inthe5daysafterthesameupwardcrossing.Vt−1refers

tothemovingaverageparameterandε2

t−1standsfortheGARCHparameter. * Significanceat10%level.

** Significanceat5%level.

(9)

Table11 GARCHanalysis:varianceequation(10days).

10Days ˛1 ε2t1 Vt−1 BD AD BU AU

Greece Coefficient 5.00E−10

** 0.0893** 0.9169** 1.00E08** 4.00E09 8.00E09** 1.00E09

p-Value 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.003 0.338 0.003 0.721

Italy Coefficient 4.00E−12

** 0.1382** 0.8549** 3.00E12** 8.00E12** 5.00E12** 1.00E12*

p-Value 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.007 0.000 0.000 0.065

Portugal Coefficient 4.00E−11

** 0.134** 0.8648** 3.00E11 8.00E11** 6.00E11** 2.00E11

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.169 0.000 0.000 0.323

Spain Coefficient 1.00E−09

** 0.1904** 0.7428** 4.00E10** 1.00E10* 1.00E09** 2.00E10**

p-Value 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.092 0.000 0.000

The table shows the results of the variance equation of a GARCH estimation of the form Rt=ˇ1+ˇ2BD+ˇ3AD+ˇ4BU+ˇ5AU+εt;

εtN(0,Vt);Vt=˛1+˛2BD+˛3AD+˛4BU+˛5AU+˛6Vt−1+˛7ε2t−1+t.BD,AD,BUandAUaredummyvariables.BDtakesthevalue

1inthe10daysbeforecrossingabarrieronadownwardmovementandzerootherwise,whereasADisforthe10daysafterthesame event.BUisforthe10daysbeforecrossingabarrierfrombelow,whileAUis1inthe10daysafterthesameupwardcrossing.Vt−1refers

tothemovingaverageparameterandε2

t−1standsfortheGARCHparameter. * Significanceat10%level.

**Significanceat1%level.

Table12 Barrierhypothesistests(5days).

5Days H1 H2 H3 H4

Greece Chi-square 3.798

* 0.580 17.303*** 13.433***

p-Value 0.051 0.446 0.000 0.000

Italy Chi-square 1.791 0.199 5.777

** 8.949***

p-Value 0.181 0.656 0.016 0.003

Portugal Chi-square 0.531 5.635

** 6.214** 0.507

p-Value 0.466 0.018 0.013 0.476

Spain Chi-square 0.198 1.078 0.251 15.568

***

p-Value 0.657 0.299 0.617 0.000

ThetableshowstheresultsofaChi-squaretestoffourdifferentnullhypotheses.H1:Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalmean returnbeforeandafteradownwardcrossingofabarrier.H2:Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalmeanreturnbeforeandafteran upwardcrossingofabarrier.H3:Thereisnodifferenceinconditionalvariancebeforeandafteradownwardcrossingofabarrier.H4: Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalvariancebeforeandafteranupwardcrossingofabarrier.

* Significanceat10%level.

**Significanceat5%level.

*** Significanceat1%level.

windowinGARCHanalysisofpsychologicalbarriersmaybe ofimportance.

Table12showsthetestresultsofthefourbarrier hypoth-esis mentioned in Section 3.2.5. If some kind of barrier indeedexisted,wewouldexpectthattherestraintsinterms of mean and variance would be relaxed after the price crossedthatbarrier.Inlinewithourpreviousanalysis, evi-denceisweakregardingconditionalmeanreturnsassociated withindices breaching abarrier. In fact, withthe excep-tionofthe Greeceand Portugalbutsolelywhen itcomes tothe returns on an upward crossing of a barrier and to adownwardcrossingofabarrier,respectively, thereis no significantchangeintheconditionalmeanreturnsinthose circumstances.

In fact, the first hypothesis, which tested differences inconditional meanreturns beforeandafter adownward crossingofabarrier,isonlyrejectedata10%levelforthe Greekcase, whereas the second one, whichfocus onthe upwardmovement,isrejectedforPortugalata5% statisti-callevel.Inbothcases,therewasasignificantincreasein themeanreturninthepost-crossingperiod.

Followingagainourprevious findings,evidenceismore significantregardingtheconditionalvolatilityofstock mar-ket indices. Regarding the third parameter restriction, which tested the difference in the conditional variance before and after a downward crossing of a barrier, we now find that this differenceis statistically significant at leastat10%levelfor threeoutofthefourmarketsofthe sample (the Spanish market is the exception). Regarding the dynamics of volatility tested in the fourth hypoth-esis, it can rejected the inexistence of differences in conditional variance before and after an upward breach-ing of a barrier again for three out of the markets which comprise the sample (the Portuguese stock mar-ket is now the exception). In all these cases, the results show a significant increase in variance after crossing the barrier.

(10)

Table13 Barrierhypothesistests(10days).

10Days H1 H2 H3 H4

Greece Chi-square 0.684 0.177 2.196 1.192

p-Value 0.408 0.674 0.138 0.275

Italy Chi-square 3.043

* 0.264 31.065*** 14.875***

p-Value 0.081 0.608 0.000 0.000

Portugal Chi-square 1.714 2.378 9.090

*** 1.963

p-Value 0.191 0.123 0.003 0.161

Spain Chi-square 8.594

*** 6.489** 144.569*** 79.987***

p-Value 0.003 0.011 0.000 0.000

ThetableshowstheresultsofaChi-squaretestoffourdifferentnullhypotheses.H1:Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalmean returnbeforeandafteradownwardcrossingofabarrier.H2:Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalmeanreturnbeforeandafteran upwardcrossingofabarrier.H3:Thereisnodifferenceinconditionalvariancebeforeandafteradownwardcrossingofabarrier.H4: Thereisnodifferenceintheconditionalvariancebeforeandafteranupwardcrossingofabarrier.

* Significanceat10%level.

** Significanceat5%level.

*** Significanceat1%level.

milderevidence,especiallyregardingtheGreekmarket,of significantchangesinconditionalvariance.

Overall, evidence suggeststhat, although thereareno significant effects in terms of returns in stock market indicesaroundbarrierpoints,volatilityisinfactsignificantly affectedinmostofthemarketsunderscrutiny,especiallyin theshortrun(uptofivedays).

AsimilarresultwasobtainedbyCyreeetal.(1999)for severalindicesrepresentingdevelopedstockmarkets.The authorsnoticedthattheirresult---asimultaneousincrease inconditionalreturnandadecreaseinconditionalvariance ---appearedtorepresentan‘‘aberration’’intheequilibrium risk---returnrelationship.AspointedoutalsobyAggarwaland Lucey(2007),suchfindingsposesomerelevantimplications for thepositiverisk---return relationship postulatedby the standardfinancialmodels.Asvarianceisnormallyusedasa proxyforrisk,changesinthisparametershouldbelinkedto changesinexpectedreturns.However,ourfindingssuggest that this relationship maybe biased in the case of stock marketindicesnearroundnumbers.

5.

Conclusion

Psychologicalbarriershavebeenfound toimpactfinancial marketsindifferentgeographiesandassetclasses.Dueto several behavioral biases and the consequent inability to make fully rational decisions, the average market prac-titioner is often affected, directly or indirectly, by such phenomenon.

Following the most widely used methodologies for studying psychological barriers, we provide new evidence regardingthisphenomenoninfourSouthernEuropeanstock markets.Consideringanextendedsampleperiod,we exam-inedtheexistenceofbarriersatroundnumbersinthemajor stock market indices of Greece (FTSE/ATHEX Large Cap), Italy(FTSEMIB),Portugal(PSI20)andSpain(IBEX35).

Insummary,itwaspossibletodistinguishthreetypesof situationsregardingthestockmarketsunderscrutiny.Inthe Italian stockmarket, itwasnotdetected anyevidenceof psychologicalbarriers.InthecaseoftheIberianstock mar-kets,thereisweakevidenceofbarriersatlowlevels(1-level

barrierand10-levelbarrier).Lastly,theGreekstockmarket is the one withthe strongest indications of psychological barriersnearbyroundnumbers.Ahighlysignificantbarrier wasdetectedatthehighestlevel(1000-levelbarrier). More-over,itwasobservedthatthestockmarketreturnstended tobe significantly higher when a barrier is crossedin an upwardmovementandsignificantlylowerwhenabarrieris crossedinadownwardmovement.Ourtestforconditional effectsalsoshowedthatthestock markets sufferedsome impactsintermsofvolatilityaroundbarriers.

Thesefindingsprovideevidencesupportingtheexistence ofpsychologicalbarrierswithrespecttoindexreturns.Our resultsare thus in line with earlier studies (e.g.,Koedijk andStork,1994;Cyreeetal.,1999;Bahng,2003)and sup-portthe claimthat technicalanalysisstrategies based on pricesupportandresistances canbeprofitable,atleastin somestockmarkets.Itisalsointerestingtonoticethatthe markets that aremore volatile---in oursample, theGreek marketand,toalesserextent,thePortugueseandSpanish markets--- aretheones thatexhibitgreaterindications of psychologicalbarriers.

The implications of the results presented here are somewhatproblematicforstandardrisk---returnequilibrium modelswhichpredictapositiverelationshipbetweenthese twovariables.Thefindingsregardingthebarrierhypothesis testspresentedinTables12and13,showthatinthemarkets underanalysistherewerestatisticallysignificantchangesin thevolatilitybetweenthepre-crossingandthepost-crossing periods.Changesinvariance,asaproxyforrisk,shouldof coursebeassociatedwithchangesinexpectedreturns. How-ever,thecontemporaneouschangesintheobservedreturns betweenthosetwoperiodsdonotseemtobesignificantin mostcases.Thisleadustoconcludethat therelationship betweenriskandreturnbecameweakeraround psycholog-icalbarriers,especiallyduringshortperiodsoftime(upto fivedays).

(11)

between the observed returns and the beta risk parame-ter becomes statistically non-significant, if not negative. And more recently, Savor and Wilson (2014) have shown that beta is positively related to average stock returns onlyondayswhenmacroeconomicsnewsregarding employ-ment, inflation, and interest rate are scheduled to be announced.Ontheremainingdays,betaisunrelatedoreven negatively related to average returns. The results of our study suggest an additional circumstance where the rela-tionship between risk and return tends to be weaker: in theproximityofpsychologicalbarriers(inourcase, round numbers).

There is much to be investigated about psychological barriersinfinancialmarkets.Furtheravenuesfor research mayincludetheadoptionof statisticaltestsbasedonthe assumption that prices follow specific distributions (e.g., theBenford’ Law) andthe study of the impactof salient events(e.g., afinancialcrisis) ontheprevalence of price barriers.

References

Aggarwal, R., Lucey, B.M., 2007. Psychological barriers in gold prices?ReviewofFinancialEconomicsXVI,217---230.

Bahng,S.,2003.Dopsychologicalbarriersexistinthestockprice indices?EvidencefromAsia’semergingmarkets.International AreaStudiesReviewVI,35---52.

Bertola,G.,Caballero,R.J.,1992.Targetzonesandrealignments. AmericanEconomicReviewLXXXII,520---536.

Brock,W.,Lakonishok,J.,LeBaron,B.,1992.Simpletechnical trad-ingrulesandthestochasticpropertiesofstockreturns.Journal ofFinanceXLVII,1731---1764.

Burke, S., 2001. Barriers in U.S. Benchmark Bonds, Vancouver (Unpublishedmanuscript).

Cai,B.M.,Cai,C.X.,Keasey,K.,2007.Influenceofculturalfactors onpriceclusteringandpriceresistanceinChina’sstockmarkets. AccountingandFinanceXLVII,623---664.

Chen, M., Tai, V.W., 2011. Psychological barriers and prices behaviourofTAIFEXfutures.GlobalEconomyandFinance Jour-nalIV,1---12.

Cyree,K.B.,Domian,D.L.,Louton,D.A.,Yobaccio,E.J.,1999. Evi-denceofpsychologicalbarriersintheconditionalmomentsof majorworldstockindices.ReviewofFinancialEconomicsVIII, 73---91.

De Ceuster, M.J.K., Dhaene, G., Schatteman, T., 1998. On the hypothesisofpsychologicalbarriersinstockmarketsand Ben-ford’sLaw.JournalofEmpiricalFinanceV,263---279.

DeGrauwe,P.,Decupere,D., 1992.Psychologicalbarriersinthe foreignexchange markets. JournalofInternational and Com-parativeEconomicsI,87---101.

Donaldson, R.G., 1990a. Psychological Barriers in Asset Prices, Rationality and the Efficient Market Hypothesis, vol. CXIV. PrincetonFinancialResearchCenterMemorandum.

Donaldson, R.G., 1990b.International Evidence onPsychological BarriersinAssetPricesandtheEfficientMarketHypothesis,vol. CXVI.PrincetonFinancialResearchCenterMemorandum. Donaldson,R.G.,Kim,H.Y.,1993.PricebarriersintheDowJones

industrialaverage.JournalofFinancialandQuantitativeAnalysis XXVIII,313---330.

Dorfleitner,G.,Klein,C.,2009.PsychologicalbarriersinEuropean stock markets: where are they? Global Finance Journal IXX, 268---285.

Dowling,M.,Cummins,M.,Lucey,B.M.,2016.Psychologicalbarriers inoilfuturesmarkets.EnergyEconomicsLIII,293---304. Fama,E.,French,E.F.,1998.Valueversusgrowth:theinternational

evidence.JournalofFinanceLIII,1975---1999.

Fama,E.,French,E.F.,2004.Thecapitalassetpricingmodel:theory andevidence.JournalofEconomicPerspectivesXVIII,25---46. George,T.J.,Hwang,C.,2004.The52-weekhighandmomentum

investing.JournalofFinanceLIX,2145---2176.

Jang,B.,Kim,C.,Kim,K.T.,Lee,S.,Shin,D.,2015.Psychological barriersand optionpricing. JournalofFuturesMarketsXXXV, 52---74.

Koedijk,K.G.,Stork,P.A.,1994.Shouldwecare?Psychological bar-riersinstockmarkets.EconomicsLettersXLIV,427---432. Ley,E.,Varian,H.R.,1994.Aretherepsychologicalbarriersinthe

Dow-Jonesindex?AppliedFinancialEconomicsIV,217---224. Savor,P.,Wilson,M.,2014.Assetpricing:ataleoftwodays.Journal

ofFinancialEconomicsCXIII,171---201.

Schwartz,A.L.,VanNess,B.F.,VanNess,R.A.,2004.Clusteringin thefuturesmarket:evidencefromS&P500futurescontracts. JournalofFuturesMarketsXXIV,413---428.

Shiller,R.J.,2015.IrrationalExuberance,3rded.Princeton Univer-sityPress.

Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1974. Judgmentunder uncertainty: heuristicsandbiases.ScienceCLXXXV,1124---1131.

Westerhoff,F.,2003.Anchoringandpsychologicalbarriersinforeign exchangemarkets.JournalofBehavioralFinanceIV,65---70. WorldBank,2014.WorldDevelopmentIndicatorsDatabase.World

Referencias

Documento similar

• To correct: needs to observe a hot star (no intrinsic absorption lines) in the same conditions (similar airmass) and divide the object’s. spectrum by the hot

In addition to provide evidence of the importance of predator behaviour on the antipredatory behaviour of birds, the results suggest that birds use the behavioural,

In order to estimate and forecast the volatility of stock index return in Amman Stock Exchange (ASE) market, four different ARCH-type models will be used, two models for testing the

1) Existence of important dynamic effects in models without explicit lags, as it happens in the case of one stock explanatory variable or in other cases, which may be

of the magnetization is generally observed in all the NP chain structures (from a to i), mainly at frontiers. Other interesting features can be observed, as an onion state in the

Interestingly in some sporadic cases TI-Her2/Mad2 tumors metastasized very severely (up to 60% of metastatic lung tissue). It would be interesting to understand whether CIN

The univariate random walk plus drift unobserved components model has the lowest RMSE for Madrid and the bivariate factor model shows the lowest value in the case of the

General mid-term adverse effects were not observed in this study, although some secondary acute reactions were observed in some individual cases (e.g. anxiety) and will be reported