• No se han encontrado resultados

The Spanish pictogram on medicines and driving: The population's comprehension of and attitudes towards its use on medication packaging.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "The Spanish pictogram on medicines and driving: The population's comprehension of and attitudes towards its use on medication packaging."

Copied!
6
0
0

Texto completo

(1)

j ou rna l h o me pag e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / a a p

The

Spanish

pictogram

on

medicines

and

driving:

The

population’s

comprehension

of

and

attitudes

towards

its

use

on

medication

packaging

Inmaculada

Fierro, Trinidad

Gómez-Talegón,

F.

Javier

Alvarez

PharmacologyandTherapeutics,FacultyofMedicine,UniversityofValladolid,C/RamónyCajal7,47005Valladolid,Spain

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory: Received9March2012

Receivedinrevisedform3August2012 Accepted6August2012

Keywords: Automobiledriving Drugprescription Drugutilization Patientinformation Pictograms Riskassessment

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

TheSpanishpictogramonmedicinesanddrivingislegallybinding since2011.Wehaveevaluated patients’comprehension,changeindrivingfrequencyandtheperceivedusefulness,information, com-prehensibility,andsimplicityofthispictogramon1385SpaniardsusersoftheNationalHealthService (pharmacies,primarycareandhospitalcentres).Most,85.7%,correctlyrelatedthesymbolwiththe pos-sibleeffectsofthemedicineondrivingandthe83.9%ofthedriverswouldreducethefrequencywith whichtheydrivewhenprescribedamedicinewithsuchpictogram.Thepictogramwasfound,ina 10-pointLikert,useful(8.3±1.7),informative(7.7±1.9),comprehensible(7.8±1.9)andsimple(7.8±1.9). TheSpanishpictogramonmedicinesanddrivingisunderstoodbythegreatmajorityofthose inter-viewed;iswellconsideredbytheusersoftheNationalHealthService;andoffersgoodprospectsfor reinforcetheawarenessofhealthcareprofessionalsandpatientsontheeffectsofmedicinesondriving.

© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Theuseofmedicineshasbeenassociatedwithanincreaseinthe riskofinvolvementintrafficaccidents(Orriolsetal.,2009,2010; Engelandetal.,2007).Forseveralyears,therehavebeennumerous attemptstoclassifymedicationsaccordingtothedegreeof dete-riorationthattheymayproduceinfitnesstodrive(Raveraetal., 2012).Inordertowarnusersofmedicinesaboutthispossiblerisk, somecountries(recentexamplesareFranceandSpain)haveopted forprintingapictogramonthemedicine’spackaging(Ministèrede laSantéetdesSolidarités,2005;MinisteriodeSanidadyConsumo, 2007).

According to the United States Pharmacopoeia (USP, 2011), pharmaceuticalpictogramsare“standardizedgraphicimagesthat helpconveymedicationinstructions,precautions,and/orwarnings topatientsandconsumers”.

Theuseofsymbols/pictogramsallowsamessagetobeexpressed ina compact way,which isalsoquickly identifiableand elimi-nateslanguagebarriers.Itwould thusseemreasonabletothink thatthemostdesirablepictogramswouldbethosewithan interna-tionalvalidityandwhichareeasilycomprehendibleforthegreatest possible number of people. Both the design and evaluation of

Correspondingauthorat:PharmacologyandTherapeutics,FacultyofMedicine, University of Valladolid, Ethics ReviewBoard, Hospital Clínico Universitario, C/RamónyCajal7,47005Valladolid,Spain.Tel.:+34983423077;

fax:+34983423022.

E-mailaddress:alvarez@med.uva.es(F.J.Alvarez).

pictogramsarecomplextasks(Wogalteretal.,2002),andtheyare oftencarriedoutinseveralstagesthatevaluateeachmodificationof theoriginaldesign.Eventhoughtheproliferationofsymbolswith identicalorsimilarmeaningsmayleadtosituationsofconfusion (Daviesetal.,1998),thepresenceofpictogramsonthelabelling ofmedicinesimprovesthecomprehensionoftheinstructionsfor theuseofthemedicine(Houtsetal.,2006)andcontributestoits acceptance(DowseandEhlers,2005).

ItiscompulsoryforpharmaceuticalcompaniesintheEuropean Uniontoprovidedataconcerningtheeffectsofeachmedication onpatients’abilitytodriveandusemachines,priorto commer-cialisation(SummaryofProductCharacteristics,Section4.7)(ECD, 1983)Theinsertprovidedinamedicine’spackaging(ECD,1983) containsinformationwarningofthepossibleeffectspatientsmay noticewhendrivingorhandlingdangerousmachinery,sincethere isampleevidencethatsomeoftheadversereactionstomedications (somnolence,dizziness,blurredvision)mayconsiderablyreduce fitnesstodriveandincreasingtheriskofinvolvementinaccidents (Orriolsetal.,2009,2010;Engelandetal.,2007).However,notall usersofamedicinereadthepackageinsert(Bernardinietal.,2000; Nathanetal.,2007),andmanyusersdonotconsiderthepackage inserttobeverycomprehensible(Barrio-Cantalejoetal.,2008).

The Royal Decree 1345/2007 regulating the procedure for authorising,registering,anddispensingindustriallymanufactured medicinesforhumanusewaspublishedinSpaininNovember2007 (MinisteriodeSanidadyConsumo,2007).ThisDecreeestablishes that newlyauthorised medicines thatmaynegatively affect fit-nesstodrive,ortheabilitytohandledangerousmachinery,must includeawarningsymbol(orpictogram)onthepackaging.From

0001-4575/$–seefrontmatter© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

(2)

Fig.1. Spanishpictogramonmedicinesanddriving:picturesofsomeavailablemedicinesintheSpanishmarkedwiththelegallybindingpictogramonmedicinesanddriving.

2011onwards,allmedicinesonsaleinSpainthatmayhavean effectonfitnesstodrivehaveincludedapictogramonthe pack-aging(AEMPS,2012).The saidsymbolmusthave thefollowing characteristics(Fig.1):

(i)Aredequilateraltrianglewiththevertexintheupperpartona whitebackgroundandablackcarinsidetheredtriangle,inthe mannerofaroadtrafficsign,andthelegendbelowit,which reads:“Driving:Seepackageinsert”.

(ii)ThesizeofthePictogramshouldbeadaptedtothesizeofthe package, but in nocase shouldeach sideof thetriangle be shorterthan10mm.

Theaimofthepictogramorsymbolistoattracttheuser’s atten-tionsothathe/shewillreadthecorrespondingpatientinformation insert.Theinsert foundinthe medicationpackage (ECD, 1983) alreadycontainsawarningabouttheeffectsitmayhaveonthose whodriveorusemachinery(sectiondrivingandusingmachines). Theaimofthisstudyistoevaluatepatients’comprehensionof theSpanishpictogramconcerningmedicinesanddrivingprinted onthe packagingof certain medications, as well asto analyse theinfluencethissymbolmayhaveontheattitudeoftheusers towardsdriving(changeindrivingfrequency)andtheperceived usefulness,information,comprehensibility,andsimplicityofthis pictogram.Inthisstudydriversandnon-driverswereincludedas anypatient/medicineconsumercouldfindsuchpictograminthe medicine’spackaging.

2. Materialandmethods

2.1. Targetpopulation

Non-institutionalizedgeneralSpanishpopulation,driversand non-drivers,aged18andover,whoresideinValladolidprovince (Spain),andareusersoftheNationalHealthService(theNHSisa universalwithpublicfundinginSpain)whenthesurveywascarried

outand,therefore,possibleconsumersofmedicamentsinthenear future.

2.2. Samplesizeandsettings

Questionnaireswerefilled inthroughindividualrandomized interviewsamongthosewhoattend:(i)PrimaryHealthcareCentres in6surgeries,(ii)pre-anaesthesiasurgeryintheHospitalClínico Universitarioand(iii)consumersfromfivepharmacies.Therewere 1385validinterviewsmadein2010fromMaytoOctober.

ThisstudywasapprovedbytheClinicalResearchEthics Com-mitteeoftheFacultyofMedicineoftheUniversityofValladolid, Referencenumber2010/30.

2.3. Questionnaire

Thefollowingquestionswereincluded:

Sociodemographic variables. (i) gender, (ii) age, (iii) driving license,(iv)kilometresdrivenperyearand(v)educationallevel.

Background.Inthefollowingitem,theintervieweeswereasked iftheyknewthatsomemedicinescaninfluencefitnesstodrive.

Pictogramcomprehension.Theintervieweewasshownthe pack-agingofrealmedicationwithapictogram,acquiredinapharmacy. He/shewasthenasked,“Whatdoyouthinkthissymbolmeans?” Theopenanswers were groupedinto fivecategories, following themodelproposedinISO9186-1:2007(ISO,2007):1=correct, 2=wrong, 3=wrong and the response given is the opposite of intendedmeaning,4=theresponsegivenis“Don’tknow”,5=no responseisgiven.Thepercentageswerecalculatedoverthetotal number ofrepliesin thecategories 1–4(ISO, 2007).The oppo-sitemeaningwas,inthiscase,“nottotake,ortostoptaking,the medicineifyoudrive”.

(3)

Gender

N(%)

Male 53(18.5) 607(55.3) 660(47.7) X2=123.63;

p<0.0001

Female 234 (81.5) 491 (44.7) 725 (52.3)

Educationallevel

N(%)

Didnotfinishprimaryschool 38(13.4) 49(4.5) 87(6.3)

X42=100.75;

p<0.0001 Finishedprimaryschool 144(50.9) 334(30.4) 478(34.6)

Finishedsecondaryschool 38(13.4) 142(12.9) 180(13.0) Completed“A”level(age18) 35(12.4) 260(23.7) 295(21.4) Universitydegree/diploma 28(9.9) 312(28.4) 340(24.6)

Age:Mean±SD(N) 55.23±17.61

(287)

47.27±14.72 (1098)

50.50±15.55 (1385)

T=5.777;

p<0.0001 Thousandsofkm/year:

Mean±SD(N)

14.83±26.23 (952)

fortheanswer:“highrisk”;“moderaterisk”;“lowrisk”;“norisk” and“Idon’tknow”.

Intendedchangeofbehaviour–changeindrivingfrequency.“How frequentlywouldyoudriveduringtheperiodinwhichyouwere takingthemedicine?”Theoptionsforansweringwere:“withthe samefrequency”;“lessfrequently”;“alotlessfrequently”;“Iwould hardlydriveatall”and“Iwouldnotdriveatall”.

Evaluation of the pictogram. Four aspects of the pictogram shown are scored separately from 1 to 10 (10-point Likert scale): “usefulness” (1=unnecessary to 10=useful); “informa-tion”(1=notinformativeto10=informative);“comprehensibility” (1=notunderstandableto10=comprehensible);and“simplicity” (1=complexto10=simple).Theintervieweeswerealsoaskedfor aglobalevaluationofthepictogramshown,onascaleof1 (mini-mum)to10(maximum).

2.4. Statisticalanalysis

Forthecontinuousvariables,themean±SDandthefrequencies forthecategoryvariablesareshown.Thecomparisonsbetween twogroupsweremadethroughthe“Studentt”inthecaseofthe continuousvariables,and forthecategoricalvariablesPearson’s chi-squaretestwasused.

Logistic regression was used to determine the sociodemo-graphicvariablesthatinfluencecomprehensionofthepictogram. Thevariablewith5categories(accordingtoISO9186-1:2007)(ISO, 2007)wasrecodedasadichotomyvariableinordertocomparethe groupthatansweredcorrectlywiththerest.Theindependent vari-ablesintroducedintotheanalysiswere:age,gender,educational level,drivinglicense,knowledgethatsomemedicinescaninfluence driving,andpriorknowledgeofthepictogram.

Logisticregressionwasalsousedtoevaluatetheimpactofthe pictogramondrivers’attitudes,analysingthepossiblechangein drivingfrequency.Todoso,the4categoriesofthevariablethat impliedadecreaseinthefrequencyofdrivingweregroupedinto one, obtainingthe dichotomy variable: Does driving frequency decreaseyesorno.Theinfluenceoftheabove-mentionedfactors wascontrolledforand,inaddition,sowasthethousandsof kilome-tresdrivenperyearandthedifferentperceptionoftheriskwhen takingamedicinewiththepictogramonitspackaging.

A10-pointLikertscalewasusedtoevaluatethepictogramin four differentparameters: usefulness, information, comprehen-sibility,and simplicity.TheCronbach’salphavaluewasusedto provethereliabilityofthescale.Theinfluenceofthevariables,age (ranges,<25;25–34;35–44;45–54;55–64;>64),gender, educa-tionallevel,andpossessingadrivinglicense,onthescoresobtained in the evaluation of theseparameters of thepictogram and in theglobalevaluation were analysed throughtheuseof a non-parametrictest(U-Mann–WhitneyorKruskal–Wallistests).

Ap-value≤0.05(95%confidenceinterval)wasconsidered sig-nificant.

The analyses were carried out using the statistical package PAWS(PredictiveAnalyticsSoftWare,v.18.0SPSSInc.,Chicago,IL, USA).Thelevelofstatisticalsignificancewasestablishedatp≤0.05.

3. Results

A total of 1385 interviews were carried out, 47.7% were men(n=660)and52.3%werewomen(n=725),withanaverage ageof50.50±15.55years(54.25±15.70yearsfor themenand 47.09±14.61yearsforthewomen).Significantdifferenceswere observedin themeanages(t=8.758[5.553;8.759];p<0.0001). 79.3%hadadrivinglicense(n=1098)and20.7%didnot(n=287).

93.9%ofthoseinterviewed(95.4%ofthedriversand88.5%of thenon-drivers,X2=18.76;p<0.05)knewthatsomemedicinescan

influencefitnesstodrive(Table1).

3.1. Pictogramcomprehension

Ofthe1363peopleinterviewedwhoansweredthequestion, “Whatdoyouthinkthesymbolmeans?”85.7%(90.5%ofthedrivers and67.4%ofthenon-drivers)correctlyrelated thesymbolwith thepossibleeffectsofthemedicineondriving.9.3%gaveawrong response:5.1%statedthattheywouldnottakethemedicineifthey weregoingtodrive,justthemeaningoppositetothatintended, and4.2%gaveotheranswers(Table2).Significantdifferenceswere observedbetweendriversandnon-driversinthecomprehension ofthepictogram(X32=115.24;p<0.0001).

Theprobabilityofacorrectinterpretationdecreasesinlinewith theincreaseinageofthoseinterviewedOR=0.969[0.957–0.980] andincreasesinlinewiththeincreasingeducationallevelofthe intervieweesOR=1.213[1.047–1.405].Theseprobabilitiesincrease amongthosewhohaveadrivinglicenseOR=3.268[2.315–4.630] andamongthosewhoknowthatsomemedicinesmayaffectfitness todriveOR=2.004[1.163–3.448].

3.2. Estimationofthelevelofdangerofthepictogramon medicinesanddriving

(4)

Table2

Spanishpictogramonmedicinesanddriving:patients’comprehension,perceivedlevelofdangerandintendedchangeindrivingfrequency.

DriverlicenseN(%) X2;p

No Yes Total

Doyouknowthatsomemedicinescaninfluencefitnesstodrive?

Yes 254(88.5) 1047(95.4) 1301(93.9)

X2=18.76;p<0.0001

No 33(11.5) 51(4.6) 84(6.1)

Whatdoyouthinkthesymbolmeans?

Correctunderstanding 190(67.4) 978(90.5) 1168(85.7)

X32=115.24;p<0.0001

Wrong:Anyotherresponse 22(7.8) 35(3.2) 57(4.2)

Wrong:Themeaningwhichisstatedistheoppositetothatintended 26(9.2) 43(4.0) 69(5.1) Theresponsegivenis:“Don’tKnow” 44(15.6) 25(2.3) 69(5.1)

Howwouldyouevaluatethedegreeofinfluenceofthismedicineondriving,i.e.,theriskyourunusingthismedicinewhendriving?

Highrisk 172 (60.1) 495 (45.2) 667 (48.3)

X42=41.77;p<0.0001

Moderaterisk 71(24.8) 397(36.3) 468(33.9)

Lowrisk 11(3.8) 51(4.7) 62(4.5)

Withoutrisk 0(0.0) 9(0.8) 9(0.7)

Don’tKnow 32(11.2) 142(13.0) 174(12.6)

Supposingyouwereprescribedthismedicinewhichhasthepictogramconcerningdrivingonthepackaging.Howfrequentlywouldyoudriveduring theperiodinwhichyouweretakingthemedicine?

Withthesamefrequency 24(8.6) 172(16.1) 196(14.6)

X42=41.78;p<0.0001

Lessfrequently 46 (16.4) 213 (20.0) 259 (19.2)

Alotlessfrequently 23(8.2) 157(14.7) 180(13.4)

Iwouldhardlydriveatall 58(20.7) 232(21.7) 290(21.5)

Iwouldnotdriveatall 129(46.1) 293(27.5) 422(31.3)

3.3. Intendedchangeofbehaviour

Changeindrivingfrequency.AsshowninTable2,only14.6% ofthoseinterviewedwouldnotreducetheirdrivingfrequencyif theywereprescribedamedicinewithapictogramaboutdrivingon thepackaging.Differenceswereobservedbetweenthetwogroups; thedriversbeinglessinclinedtochangetheirhabits(X42=41.78; p<0.0001).Thedecreaseinthefrequencyofdrivingamongdrivers ismoreprobabletheolderthedriveis,OR=1.022[1.008–1.037], andlessprobablethemore kilometrestheynormallydroveper year,OR=0.989[0.983–0.994].Furthermore,takinginto considera-tionthosewhobelievedthatdrivingaftertakingofamedicinewith apictogram“didnotimplyanyriskorthattheriskwaslow”,the greatertheprobabilityofdecreaseinthefrequencyofdrivingifthe attributedriskis“veryhigh”,OR=16.500[8.340–32.645],orwhen theriskisconsideredtobe“moderate”,OR=3.557[1.923–6.577].

3.4. Evaluationofthepictogram

Thepictogramwasfoundtobeuseful,informative, comprehen-sibleand simple.Table3showstheaveragescores(Mean±SD) giventodifferentparametersofthepictogram(usefulness, infor-mation,comprehensibilityandsimplicity).TheCronbach’salpha valueof0.837provesthereliabilityofthe10-pointLikertscaleto evaluatetheseparametersofthepictogram.

Theinfluenceofthevariablesage,gender,educationallevel,and possessingadrivinglicenseonthescoresobtainedinthe evalua-tionofusefulness,information,comprehensibility,andsimplicity wasanalysedusingnon-parametrictests(Table3).Genderhadno influenceonthescores.Significantdifferenceswereobservedinthe meanscoregiventothedifferentaspectsofthepictogram accord-ingtotheagerangeofthoseinterviewed:thehighestmeanswere observedforallthescoresinthoseintervieweesaged25–34and 35–44,whilethelowestmeanswereobservedinthoseagedover 64.Ingeneral,thehighertheeducationlevel,thehigherthescores ofalltheparameters ofthepictogram.Thedriversgave higher scoresthanthenon-driverstothecomprehensibilityandsimplicity ofthepictogram(Table3).

AsfortheGlobalEvaluationofthepictogram,theaveragescore obtainedwas7.98±1.58points.Theglobalscoregiventothe Span-ishpictogramisindependentof:theageofthoseinterviewed,their

gender,theireducationallevel,andwhetherornottheypossessed adrivinglicense(Table3).

4. Discussion

TheresultsofthestudyshowthattheSpanishpictogramon medicinesanddrivingis,ingeneral,wellvaluedbytheusersof medicinesanditsmeaningiscomprehendedbyahigh percent-ageofthoseinterviewed.Thisisespeciallytrueamongthosewho possessadrivinglicense,acircumstancethatisfavourableasthis questionmainlyaffectsdrivers.Thebettercomprehensionofthe pictogrambydriversmaymostlybeduetothedesigntypeofthe pictogram,whichiscloselyrelatedtothatoftrafficsigns.Although inpreviousstudies,ithasalsobeenobservedthatdriving experi-enceimprovesaperson’scapacitytointerpretothertypesofsigns, suchas,forinstance,thoserelatedwithindustrialsafety(Chanand Ng,2010).

Ontheotherhand,itmustbesaidthatwhentheinterviewswere held(MaytoOctober2010)fewmedicineshadthispictogramon thepackaging,and,therefore,ithadnotbeenwidelyseenamong thepopulation.Thisoffersagoodperspectivetothesymbol’s use-fulness,since,asobservedinvariousstudies,familiaritywiththe signsimprovescomprehension(Daviesetal.,1998;Easterbyand Hakiel,1981).Therefore,itisreasonabletoexpectthatthe com-prehensionofthissymbolamongthepopulationwillincreaseasit becomesmorewidelyusedandisincludedinpublicitycampaigns. Ourstudyrevealsthattheprobabilityofcorrectlyinterpreting thispictogramdecreasesastheageofthoseinterviewedincreases, andit increasesiftheintervieweehasahighertheeducational level.Italsoincreasesifthoseinterviewedhadpriorknowledge ofmedicinesthatcaninfluencefitnesstodrive.

(5)

Gender Male 8.25±1.72 7.69±1.87 7.79±1.86 7.80±1.94 7.90±1.59 Female 8.37±1.67 7.69±1.93 7.74±1.96 7.80±1.93 8.06±1.56

UMann–Whitney;p 223,189.50;p>0.05 231,618.00;p>0.05 228,845.00;p>0.05 229,331.50;p>0.05 210,161.00;p>0.05

Drivinglicence No 8.19±1.71 7.45±1.88 7.36±2.01 7.41±2.01 8.00±1.60 Yes 8.35±1.69 7.75±1.90 7.87±1.87 7.90±1.91 7.98±1.57

UMann–Whitney;p 223,189.50;p>0.05 231,618.00;p<0.01 228,845.00;p<0.0001 229,331.50;p<0.0001 210,161.50;p>0.05

Agerange <25 8.28±1.76 7.44±2.03 7.54±1.99 8.03±1.71 7.85±1.20 25–34 8.50±1.67 7.96±1.83 8.27±1.66 8.15±1.93 8.10±1.32 35–44 8.59±1.60 7.80±1.87 8.01±1.87 8.11±1.82 8.10±1.53 45–54 8.28±1.71 7.72±1.95 7.69±1.98 7.78±1.87 7.97±1.67 55–64 8.35±1.59 7.69±1.94 7.73±1.87 7.80±1.85 7.93±1.95 >64 7.92±1.82 7.43±1.84 7.36±1.96 7.24±2.13 7.88±1.67 Kruskal–Wallis;p 31.73;p<0.01 20.20;p<0.05 44.11;p<0.0001 46.16;p<0.0001 5.99;p>0.05

EducationalLevel Didnotfinishprimaryschool 7.49±1.88 7.36±1.73 7.32±1.96 7.02±2.25 7.93±1.91 Finishedprimaryschool 8.29±1.64 7.49±1.93 7.43±1.99 7.43±1.97 7.91±1.63 Finishedsecondaryschool 8.44±1.63 7.84±1.86 8.10±1.78 8.12±1.83 8.06±1.40 Completed“A”level(age18) 8.36±1.73 7.75±1.91 7.92±1.88 7.95±1.93 8.07±1.60 Universitydegree/diploma 8.44±1.70 7.91±1.89 8.02±1.82 8.20±1.71 7.97±1.47 Kruskal–Wallis;p 25.45;p<0.0001 17.42;p<0.01 35.21;p<0.0001 53.81;p<0.0001 2.52;p>0.05

Meanscores(±SD).ForeachCharacteristicandfortheglobalscorea10-pointLikertscalewasused(1:negative;10:positive).

consumers,observedthatconsumersovertheage55hadalower comprehensionofthesymbolsonthelabelsofdifferentproducts. Therearealsonumerousstudiesshowingthatcomprehension ofgraphicmessagesand,inparticular,comprehensionofwarning labelsonmedicinesimprovesinlinewiththeeducationallevelof theinterviewee(DowseandEhlers,2003;Davisetal.,2006). How-ever,itispreciselythosewithalowereducationallevelwhowould mostbenefitfromtheuseofpictograms(Michielutteetal.,1992). Pictogramscanbeofgreathelpintheacceptanceofmedication andinthecomprehensionoftheuseofaprescriptionforthose patientswithlowerlevelsofeducation,evenforthoseunableto readandwrite(DowseandEhlers,2005;Davisetal.,2006;Dowse andEhlers,2001).

Inourcase,andinviewoftheresults,thepresenceofthe pic-togramonmedicationpackagingmayplayafundamentalrolein changingdrivers’attitudes(83.9%ofthedriverswouldreducetheir drivingfrequency).ThesamewasnotobservedinaDutchstudy carriedoutwithdriverssufferingfromchronicpain.Inthiscase, theauthorsconcludedthatthewarninglabelsdidnotsignificantly modifytheattitudetowardsdrivingofpatientswhoweretaking medicineswithapsychotropiceffect(Veldhuijzenetal.,2006).

Ithasbeenobservedthatagreaterperceptionofriskimproved thedispositiontoreadwarningmessages(Wogalteretal.,1991, 1993),althoughthefinaldecisioncouldbeinfluencedmorebythe person’sownperceptionoftheriskthanbythewarningmessage (Daviesetal.,1998).Daviesetal.(Davisetal.,2006)demonstrate theinfluenceoftheperceptionofrisk whentheyobservedthat parents’finaldecisionsonwhethertopurchaseorrejectatoywere influencedmorebytheperceptionoftheproduct’s dangerthan bythewarningmessageonthepackaging.Theinfluenceofthe attributedriskofthepictogram,whichwasalsoshowninthisstudy, isanimportantvariablewhenconsideringaperson’swillingness tochangetheirattitudetowardsdriving.Thisfactordeservestobe takenintoaccountgiventhattheinterpretationoftheriskinthe Spanishpictogramcouldbehighlysubjective.Thisholdstruefor allmedicinesinregardstoinformingpatientsabouttheeffectsthat amedicationmayhaveonapatient’sfitnesstodrive.

Ourstudyhadseveralpotentiallimitations.Thisstudywas con-ceivedasanexploratorysurveytoevaluatethecomprehensibility ofthepictogramonmedicinesanddriving,whichiswhyitwas doneinalimitedgeographicalarea.Therefore,theresultsshould

notbeextrapolatedtothenationallevel.Furthermore,atthetime thesurveyswerebeingcarriedout,onlythepackagingofmedicines forthecentralnervoussystemhadbeenrevisedtoincludethe pic-togram.Thismayhavehadaninfluenceonsomeoftheresults, especiallyintheestimationofthelevelofdanger.Inthisstudy,we havetriedtoprovideanadequatecontextinwhichtoshowthe symbol,since,assomestudieshaveshown,comprehensionofa symboldependsonthecontext(Lehto,2000).However,thismeans thatthesymbol’s capacitytoattractattentioncannotbe evalu-atedsincethemedicine’spackagingwasshowntotheinterviewee asencouragementtoobservethepictogramwhenconductingthe survey.Thisfactcouldhavediminishedthesymbol’sreal effec-tivenessofreducingthenumberofaccidentsbecauseifithadnot beennoticed,itwouldnothavehadanaffectatallonthedriver’s attitude. On the other hand,since the symbolis already being usedonmedicationpackaging,certainfactorssuchastheinfluence ofthesize(ShiehandHuang,2003)thesetting,thedesign(ISO, 2004)etc.,ontheinterpretationofthepictogramhavenotbeen analysed.

5. Conclusion

TheSpanishpictogramonmedicinesanddrivingisunderstood bythegreatmajorityoftheinterviewees,iswellvaluedbyusersof theNationalHealthService.Thispictogramcanbeseenasatoolto improveprescribinganddispensingproceduresofmedicinesthat impairdrivingaswellasaninstrumenttomakepatientsawareof theroleofmedicinesplayintrafficsafety.

Conflictofinterest

Theauthorsdeclarednoconflictofinterest.

Acknowledgements

(6)

TheauthorsoftheSpanishstudywouldliketothankthepatients and health professionals (physicians, pharmacists and nursing staff)involvedinthestudyfortheirparticipation.Wewouldalso liketothankthehealthauthorities(JuntadeCastillayLeón, Conse-jeríadeSanidad,Sacyl,MinisteriodeSanidadyConsumo–Agencia Espa ˜noladeMedicamentosyProductosSanitarios(AEMPS)),the PrimaryHealth Care Centers, theHospital Clínico Universitario ofValladolid,theColegioOficialdeFarmacéuticosdeValladolid, SEMT,andSETfortheircollaborationatalltimes.Specialthanksto LurdesRicofortheircontributiontothestudy.

References

AgenciaEspa ˜noladeMedicamentosyProductosSanitarios,2012. Medicamen-tosyConducciónhttp://www.aemps.gob.es/industria/etiquetado/conduccion/ home.htm(January2012).

Barrio-Cantalejo,I.M.,Simon-Lorda,P.,MarchCerda,J.C.,PrietoRodriguez,M.A., 2008.Grammaticalreadabilityofthepackageleafletsofthemedicinal prod-uctsmostwidelyconsumedandgeneratingthehighestexpenseinSpainduring 2005.RevistaEspa ˜noladeSaludPública82,559–566.

Bernardini,C.,Ambrogi,V.,Perioli,L.C.,Tiralti,M.C.,Fardella,G.,2000. Compre-hensibilityofthepackageleafletsofallmedicinalproductsforhumanuse:a questionnairesurveyabouttheuseofsymbolsandpictograms.Pharmacological Research41,679–688.

Chan,A.H.S.,Ng,A.W.Y.,2010.Investigationofguessabilityofindustrialsafetysigns: effectsofprospective-userfactorsandcognitivesignfeatures.International JournalofIndustrialErgonomics40,689–697.

Davies,S.,Haines,H.,Norris,B.,Wilson,J.R.,1998.Safetypictograms:aretheygetting themessageacross?AppliedErgonomics29,15–23.

Davis,T.C.,Wolf,M.S.,Bass,P.F.,Middlebrooks,M.,Kennen,E.,Baker,D.W.,Bennett, C.L.,Durazo-Arvizu,R.,Bocchini,A.,Savory,S.,Parker,R.M.,2006.Lowliteracy impairscomprehensionofprescriptiondrugwarninglabels.JournalofGeneral InternalMedicine21,847–851.

Dowse,R.,Ehlers,M.,2005.Medicinelabelsincorporatingpictograms:dothey influenceunderstandingandadherence?PatientEducationandCounseling58, 63–70.

Dowse,R.,Ehlers,M.S.,2001.Theevaluationofpharmaceuticalpictogramsina low-literateSouthAfricanpopulation.PatientEducationandCounseling45, 87–99.

Dowse,R.,Ehlers,M.S.,2003.Theinfluenceofeducationontheinterpretationof pharmaceuticalpictogramsforcommunicatingmedicineinstructions. Interna-tionalJournalofPharmacyPractice11,11–18.

Easterby,R.S., Hakiel,S.R., 1981. Field testing of consumer safety signs: the comprehensionof pictoriallypresentedmessages.AppliedErgonomics12, 143–152.

Engeland,A.,Skurtveit,S.,Morland,J.,2007.Riskofroadtrafficaccidents associ-atedwiththeprescriptionofdrugs:aregistry-basedcohortstudy.Annalsof Epidemiology17,597–602.

EuropeanCouncilDirective83/570/EECof26October1983onthe approxima-tion of provisionslaid down by law, regulation or administrative action relatingto proprietary medicinal products,1983. http://eur-lex.europa.eu/ LexUriServ/LexUriServ.do?uri=CELEX:31983L0570:EN:HTML(January2012).

Hancock,H.E.,Fisk, A.D.,Rogers,W.A.,2005.Comprehendingproductwarning information:age-relatedeffectsandtherolesofmemory,inferencing,and knowledge.HumanFactors47,219–234.

Houts,P.S.,Doak,C.C.,Doak,L.G.,Loscalzo,M.J.,2006.Theroleofpicturesin improv-inghealthcommunication.Areviewofresearchonattention,comprehension, recall,andadherence.PatientEducationandCounseling61,173–190. ISO3864-2:2004,2004.GraphicalSymbols–SafetyColoursandSafetySigns–Part

2:DesignPrinciplesforProductSafetyLabels.ISO3864-2.

ISO9186-1:2007,2007.GraphicalSymbols–TestMethods–Part1:Methodsfor TestingComprehensibility.ISO9186-1.

Lehto,M.R.,2000.Designingwarningsignsandwarninglabels:partII–scientific basisforinitialguidelines.In:AnilMital,ÅsaKilbom,ShrawanKumar(Eds.), ElsevierErgonomicsBookSeries.,pp.257–280.

Michielutte,R.,Bahnson,J.,Dignan,M.B.,Schroeder,E.M.,1992.Theuseof illus-trationsandnarrativetextstyletoimprovereadabilityofahealtheducation brochure.JournalofCancerEducation7,251–260.

MinistèredelaSantéetdesSolidarités,2005.Arrêtédu18Juillet2005prispour lˇıapplicationdelˇıarticleR.5121-139ducodedelasantépubliqueetrelative àlˇıoppositiondˇıunpictogrammesurleconditionnementextérieurdecertain médicamentsetproduits.JournalOfficieldelaRépubliqueFranc¸aise,2Août 2005(SAN/P0522726A).

MinisteriodeSanidad yConsumo,2007.REAL DECRETO1345/2007,de 11de octubre,porelqueseregulaelprocedimiento deautorización,registroy condicionesdedispensacióndelosmedicamentosdeusohumanofabricados industrialmente.BOE267,45652-45698.

Nathan,J.P.,Zerilli,T.,Cicero,L.A.,Rosenberg,J.M.,2007.Patients’useandperception ofmedicationinformationleaflets.AnnalsofPharmacotherapy41,777–782. Orriols,L.,Delorme,B.,Gadegbeku,B.,Tricotel,A.,Contrand,B.,Laumon,B.,Salmi,

L.R.,Lagarde,E.,CESIRresearchgroup,2010.Prescriptionmedicinesandthe riskofroadtrafficcrashes:aFrenchregistry-basedstudy.PLoSMedicine7, e1000366.

Orriols,L.,Salmi,L.R.,Philip,P.,Moore,N.,Delorme,B.,Castot,A.,Lagarde,E.,2009. Theimpactofmedicinaldrugsontrafficsafety:asystematicreviewof epidemi-ologicalstudies.PharmacoepidemiologyandDrugSafety18,647–658. Ravera, S.,Monteiro,S.P., deGier,J.J., vander Linden,T., Gómez-Talegón, T.,

Alvarez,F.J.,theDRUIDProjectWP4Partners,2012.AEuropeanapproachto categorisingmedicinesforfitnesstodrive:outcomesoftheDRUIDproject. BritishJournalofClinicalPharmacology(March),http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j. 1365-2125.2012.04279.x

EpubaheadofprintPubMedPMID:22452358.

Shieh,K.,Huang,S.,2003.Factorsaffectingpreferenceratingsofprohibitivesymbols. AppliedErgonomics34,581–587.

United States Pharmacopoeia,2011. http://www.usp.org/audiences/consumers/ pictograms/(December2011).

Veldhuijzen,D.S.,vanWijck,A.J.,Verster,J.C.,Kalkman,C.J.,Kenemans,J.L.,Olivier, B.,Volkerts,E.R.,2006.Theimpactofchronicpainpatients’psychotropicdrug knowledgeandwarninglabelsonthedecisionwhethertodriveacarornot. TrafficInjuryPrevention7,360–364.

Wogalter,M.S.,Brelsford,J.W.,Desaulniers,D.R.,Laughery,K.R.,1991.Consumer productwarnings:theroleofhazardperception.JournalofSafetyResearch22, 71–82.

Wogalter,M.S.,Brems,D.J.,Martin,E.G.,1993.Riskperceptionofcommonconsumer products:judgmentsofaccidentfrequencyandprecautionaryintent.Journalof SafetyResearch24,97–106.

Figure

Fig. 1. Spanish pictogram on medicines and driving: pictures of some available medicines in the Spanish marked with the legally binding pictogram on medicines and driving.

Referencias

Documento similar

No obstante, como esta enfermedad afecta a cada persona de manera diferente, no todas las opciones de cuidado y tratamiento pueden ser apropiadas para cada individuo.. La forma

The aim of the this study is to evaluate the hemodynamic changes of blood pressure and heart rate on hypertensive patients receiving three different types of local anesthe-

Thus, the aim of this study was to evaluate the influence of the type of management system on goat kid meat quality as well as to assess the fatty acid profile of the extensive

In the preparation of this report, the Venice Commission has relied on the comments of its rapporteurs; its recently adopted Report on Respect for Democracy, Human Rights and the Rule

The draft amendments do not operate any more a distinction between different states of emergency; they repeal articles 120, 121and 122 and make it possible for the President to

In the previous sections we have shown how astronomical alignments and solar hierophanies – with a common interest in the solstices − were substantiated in the

teriza por dos factores, que vienen a determinar la especial responsabilidad que incumbe al Tribunal de Justicia en esta materia: de un lado, la inexistencia, en el

We analysed the prevalence of gross (GH) and microscopic (mH) haematuria in 19,895 patients that underwent native renal biopsies from the Spanish Registry of Glomerulonephritis..