The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context
w w w . e l s e v i e r . e s / e j p a l
Predictors of sexual aggression in adolescents: Gender dominance vs.
rape supportive attitudes
Nieves Moyano
a, Fredy S. Monge
b, Juan Carlos Sierra
c,∗aUniversidaddeEspecialidadesEspírituSanto,Guayaquil,Ecuador
bUniversidadNacionaldeSanAntonioAbaddelCusco,Perú
cCentrodeInvestigaciónMente,CerebroyComportamiento(CIMCYC),UniversidaddeGranada,Espa˜na
a r t i c l e i n f o
Articlehistory:
Received19February2016
Receivedinrevisedform10June2016 Accepted13June2016
Availableonline27July2016
Keywords:
Sexualaggression Sexualdoublestandard Rapeattitudes Adolescents
a b s t r a c t
Thisstudyexploredtherelationshipbetweensexualdoublestandardandrapesupportiveattitudesin regardtoanindividual’slikelihoodtoperpetratesexualaggression.Weexaminedanadolescentsample of448boysfromPeru,ofwhom148(33.3%)reportedtohavecommittedsexualaggression.Sexual contactwithanunwillingpartnerwasperpetratedby24.8%ofthetotalsample,sexualcoercionby14.3%, attemptedrapeby12.5%,andfinally,rapewasperpetratedby10.3%.Inallthesetypesofaggression,the mostfrequentvictimwasadatingpartner.Comparedtonon-aggressors,maleaggressorsreportedmore sexualdoublestandardandsupportiveattitudestowardsrape.Logisticregressionanalysesrevealedthat themostrelevantvariableinthepredictionofsexualaggressionwasthesubjecthavingbeenavictimof sexualabuseduringadolescenceandhavingrapesupportiveattitudes.Ourfindingssuggestthatviolent attitudesaremoreimportantthantheendorsementofnon-egalitarianbeliefs(sexualdoublestandard) intheperpetrationofsexualviolence.ThesefindingsprovidedatafromPeru,whichcontributetothe worldwidedataonriskfactorsforsexualaggressioninadolescentmales.
©2016ColegioOficialdePsic ´ologosdeMadrid.PublishedbyElsevierEspa ˜na,S.L.U.Thisisanopen accessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-NDlicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Predictoresdelaagresiónsexualenadolescentes:dominanciadegénerovs.
actitudesfavorableshacialaviolación
Palabrasclave:
Agresiónsexual Dobleestándarsexual Actitudeshacialaviolación Adolescentes
re s um e n
Esteestudioexploralarelaciónentreeldobleestándarsexualylasactitudesfavorableshacialaviolación enelriesgodellevaracabounactodeagresiónsexual.Seexaminóunamuestradeadolescentesfor- madapor448chicosdePerú,deloscuales148(33.3%)informaronhaberperpetradoagresiónsexual.En concreto,el24.8%informóhaberperpetradocontactossexualessinelconsentimientodelavíctima,el 14.3%habíacometidoactosdecoerciónsexual,el12.5%intentóllevaracaboviolaciónyel10.3%perpetró violación.Lavíctimamáscomúndeestetipodeabusosfueronlasparejasocasionales.Encomparación conelgrupoquenuncahabíaagredido,losvaronesagresoresinformarondemayordoblemoralsexual yactitudesmáspositivashacialaviolación.Unanálisisderegresiónlogísticarevelóquelavariablemás importanteenlaprediccióndelaagresiónsexualfuehabersidovíctimadeabusosexualdurantelaado- lescenciaytenerunaactitudpositivahacialaviolación.Losresultadossugierenquelasactitudeshaciala violenciasonmásimportantesquelascreenciasnoigualitarias(doblemoralsexual)enlarealizaciónde conductassexualesviolentas.Estehallazgo,alofrecerinformacióndePerú,sea ˜nadealosdatosexistentes anivelmundialsobrelosfactoresderiesgodelaagresiónsexualenadolescentesvarones.
©2016ColegioOficialdePsic ´ologosdeMadrid.PublicadoporElsevierEspa ˜na,S.L.U.Esteesun art´ıculoOpenAccessbajolalicenciaCCBY-NC-ND(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
).
∗ Correspondingauthor.FacultaddePsicología,CampusdeCartuja,s/n.18011.
Granada,Spain.
E-mailaddress:[email protected](J.C.Sierra).
Sexualaggressionisawidespreadphenomenoninsocietiesand cultures throughout theworld.Prevalence rates, however,vary fromcountrytocountry.Accordingtoastudyconductedbythe WorldHealthOrganization(WHO)withdatafromtencountries, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpal.2016.06.001
1889-1861/©2016ColegioOficialdePsic ´ologosdeMadrid.PublishedbyElsevierEspa ˜na,S.L.U.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-NDlicense(http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
from13%(Japan)to62%(Peru)ofwomenreportedphysicalvic- timizationintheirlifetime(Garcia-Moreno,Jansen,Ellsberg,Heise,
&Watts,2006).Thesedatahighlightthestrikingprevalencerates ofviolenceinLatinAmericancountries,andparticularlyinPeru.
Anotherstudy byCáceres (2005)showed that 46%of Peruvian womenaged16–30reportednon-consensualsexbyanopposite- sexperpetratoratleastonceintheirlifetime.
Thesehighrates ofsexualaggressionhavebeenobservedat younger ages. For example, research coordinated by the WHO foundthat 23% ofwomen from Limaand 47%of women from Cuzcoreportedhavingsufferedsexualvictimizationbyagefifteen (Güezmes&Vargas, 2003).More recentdatafromtheMinistry ofWomenandVulnerablePopulations(MinisteriodelaMujery PoblacionesVulnerables,MIMP,2015)showedthattherehadbeen 28,790casesoffamilyandsexualviolenceinPerufromJanuaryto June2015.Therefore,researchanalyzingtheriskfactorsassociated withhighprevalenceratesofsexualaggressionincountriessuch asPeruisbothtimelyandnecessary.
However,despitethefactthatsexualviolencepreventionpro- grams increasingly target younger ages, there has been little researchwitha sample populationof adolescents. Thisis rele- vantbecausetheendorsementofcertaindominantattitudes,such assexualdouble standard, which is oftenlinkedtodating vio- lence,aredeveloped and establishedatearlyages(Gallagher&
Parrott,2011; Greene& Faulkner,2005; Kreager&Staff, 2009;
Lyons,Giordano,Manning, &Longmore,2011; Marston&King, 2006;Sanchez,Fetterolf,&Rudman,2012).Inparticular,teenage malesareoftenexposedtopeerpressureandthustendtobehave accordingtosocialandgenderexpectations,astheydeveloptheir
“masculineidentity”(Lees,2002).Thisleadsboystobeespecially vulnerabletothedevelopmentofcertainformsofviolentbehaviors (Shen,Chiu,&Gao,2012).
Sexualdoublestandardhasbeendefinedastheappropriate- nessofcertainsexualbehaviorswhentheyareperformedbymen, thanwhentheyareperformedbywomen(Fasula,Carry,&Miller, 2014;Milhausen&Herold,2002).Inotherwords,althoughwomen are stigmatized for engaged in explicit sexualbehaviors, these behaviorsareencouragedinmeninsimilarsexualsituations.The conceptualizationof certain sexualbehaviors as appropriate or inappropriateisoftenguidedbyculturalexpectations(seeByers, 1996;Emmers-Sommeretal.,2010).Inthisregard,LatinAmerican countriesareaspecialcase,astheyoftenendorserelativelymore rigidgenderroles(Gutiérrez-Quintanilla,Rojas-García, &Sierra, 2010;Raffaelli&Ontai,2001).
Variousstudieshaveshownthatrapeattitudesplayanimpor- tant role in sexualviolence (see Sierra, Bermúdez, Buela-Casal,
&Salinas,2014),asthey“servetodenyandjustifymalesexual aggressionagainstwomen”(Lonsway&Fitzgerald,1994,p.134).
Researchonsexual offendershasconceptualizedrapeattitudes intermsofthecognitionsthatjustifywhywomen becomevic- timsof rape.AccordingtoJohnson, Kuck,and Schander(1997), mostrapemythsandrape-acceptingattitudesfitintooneofthree categories:(1)blamingthevictim,(2)excusingtheperpetratorof responsibilityfortheassault,and(3)justificationfortheassault.
Theassociationbetweensupportiverapeattitudesandtheperpe- trationofsexualaggression,throughself-reportedmeasures,has beendemonstratedincollegestudentsandcommunitysamplesof maleadults(i.e.,DeGue,DeLillo,&Scalora,2010;Helmus,Hanson, Thornton,Babchishin,&Harris,2012).Furthermore,adolescents areavulnerablepopulation,asrapeisoftenexperiencedforthe firsttimeintheseyears(Jewkes&Sikweyiya,2013).
Adirectpositiverelationshipwasfoundbetweenvictimization andrelationalviolencebehaviorinadolescents(Povedano,Cava, Monreal,Varela,&Musitu,2015).Researchindicatesthatprevious experiencesofsexualvictimizationareoftenassociatedwiththe subsequentperpetrationofsexualviolence,asshown incollege
men(e.g.,Russell&Oswald,2002)andcollegewomen(Russell&
Oswald,2001;Shea,1998).Thisisknownasthecycleofsexualcoer- cion(Gannon,Rose,&Ward,2008;Moyano&Sierra,2015;Ward&
Beech,2006).FindingsfromtheNationalLongitudinalStudyofAdo- lescentHealthreflectthatvictimizationinadolescentsispredictive oflaterperpetrationofviolenceinadulthood(Gomez,Speizer,&
Moracco,2011).
The present study investigated the sexual double standard andrapesupportiveattitudes,factorsthatmaycontributetoan increasedriskoftheperpetrationofsexualaggressioninasample ofadolescentmalesfromPeru.Inaddition,theroleofself-reported adolescentsexualvictimizationwasexplored.Theconsideration ofthesevariablesisfundamentalforpromotingsexualhealthpro- gramstargetingthispopulation.Culturalvaluesshapeconceptions ofhealthysexuality,andculturalpracticespromotetheseidealsin development(Manago,Greenfield,Kim,&Ward,2014).
Therefore,thisstudyhadthefollowingobjectives:(1)tocom- pareboyswhoreportedtohaveperpetratedsexualaggressionwith boyswhoreportedtohave not,basedontheirendorsementof sexualdoublestandardandsupportiveattitudestowardsrape;(2) todeterminewhichofthesefactors(sexualdoublestandardorsup- portiveattitudestowardsrape)bestpredictssexualaggressionin boys.
Previous research on the adult population has shown that, comparedtoothermen,thosewhoreportmoretraditionalmas- culinityideologiesaremore likelytoreporthavingperpetrated violenceorsexualcoercion(Marín,Gómez,Tschann,&Gregorich, 1997;Santana,Raj,Decker,LaMarche,&Silverman,2006;Sierra, Gutiérrez-Quintanilla,Bermúdez,&Buela-Casal,2009).Conversely, incomparisontolessegalitarianmen,menwithmoreegalitarian genderroleideologiesreportfewerinstancesofphysicalaggression againsttheirintimatepartners(Fitzpatrick,Salgado,Suvak,King,&
King,2004).Therefore,thefollowinghypotheseswereproposed andtested:
H1. Boyswhoreportsexualaggressionwillreportmoresexual doublestandardandamoresupportiveattitudetowardsrapethan non-aggressors.
H2. Self-reportedsexualaggressionwillbepredictedbysexual doublestandardandsupportiveattitudetowardsrape.
Method
Participants
Werecruitedaninitialsamplecomposedof500Peruvianmale adolescents,agedbetween14and17years.Datafrom52partic- ipants wereexcludedbecauseof missing values (i.e.,whenthe questionnaire was incomplete or more than 75% of the items had not been answered). This left a final sample of 448. Par- ticipantswererecruitedbyincidentalsamplingfromonepublic secondaryschoolinCusco,Peru.Schoolwasonlyforboysfroma lowtomediumsocioeconomiclevelfamilies.Ofthesample,33%of participants(n=148)reportedhavingengagedinsexualaggres- sionatleastonce(i.e.,sexualaggressors).Asshown inTable1, therewerenosignificantdifferencesbetweensexualaggressors andindividuals who reportedtohave never perpetratedsexual aggression(i.e.,non-aggressors)inage,residencearea(urbanor rural),orreligiousattendance(fromnevertoeveryday).
Measures
WeusedaSocio-demographicQuestionnairethatcollecteddata aboutsex,age,residencearea,religiousattendance,whotheylive with,andparents’job.
Table1
Sociodemographic Characteristics of Adolescent Sexual Aggressors and Non- Aggressors.
Sexualaggressors (n=148)
Nonsexual aggressors (n=300)
t/2/U
M(SD) M(SD)
Age 16.05(0.82) 16.10(0.81) -.03
n(%) n(%)
Residence .45
Urban 129(87.2) 258(86)
Rural 14(9.5) 36(12)
Religiosity .31
Never 12(8.1) 21(7)
Yearly 42(28.4) 79(26.3)
Monthly 38(25.7) 71(23.7)
Weekly 51(34.5) 123(41)
Everyday 2(1.4) 2(0.7)
The Sexual Double Standard(SDS; Caron,Davis, Halteman, &
Stickle,1993).Thisscaleiscomposedof10itemsansweredona five-pointLikert-typescale,whichrangesfrom1(stronglydisagree) to5 (strongly agree).It assesses the endorsementof traditional sexualgenderstandards.Higherscoresindicatehigherendorse- mentof SDS.TheauthorsreportCronbach’salphaequal to.72.
TheSpanishadaptationfoundaCronbach’salphavalueof.76in asampleofmen.Thisstudyusedanadaptationofthismeasure administeredinPerubyMonge,Sierra,andSalinas(2013).These authorsproposedareduced9-item,withreliabilityvaluesof.78.
Scoreshavealsoshowntobeinvariantacrossmenandwomen.In thepresentstudy,Cronbach’salphawas.63.
TheRapeSupportiveAttitudesScale(RSAS;Lottes,1991).Weused anadaptationadministeredtoPeruvianwomenbySierra,Monge, Santos-Iglesias,Rodriguez,and Aparicio(2010).Itconsistsof20 itemsansweredonafive-pointLikerttypescale,rangingfrom1 (stronglydisagree)to5(stronglyagree).Theglobalscoreisthesum ofalltheitems.Scoresrangefrom1to100.Higherscoresindicate asupportiveattitudetowardrape.Cronbach’salphavalueofthe originalversionwas.91,andfortheSpanishversionadministered toPeruvianwomenwas.88(Sierra,Mongeetal.,2010).Cronbach’s alphavaluewhenappliedtomaleuniversitystudentsfromSpain was.92(Sierra,Rojas,Ortega,&Martín-Ortiz,2007).Inthepresent study,Cronbach’salphawas.72.
TheSexualExperiencesSurvey(SES)(Kossetal.,2007)iscom- prised of 10 items and contains two subscales. One subscale assessesthecommissionofsexualaggressionaftertheageof14.
Theothersubscaleassesseshavingexperiencedsexualvictimiza- tionafter theageof 14. Anexample of anitem for the sexual aggressivenesssubscaleis“Haveyouhadsexactswithsomeone (fondling,kisses)whenhe/shedidn’twantto,becausehe/shefelt overwhelmedbyyourargumentsandpressure?”Anexampleofthe sexualvictimizationsubscaleis“Haveyouhadsexacts(analororal intercourseorpenetrationbyobjectsotherthanthepenis)when youdidn’twanttobecausesomeonethreatenedyouorusedsome degreeofphysical force(twistingyour arm,holdingyou down, etc.).”Eachitemisansweredonasix-pointLikerttypescaleran- gingfrom0(never),1(1time)andsoonto5(5ormoretimes).This instrumentconsiders foursubtypesof sexualaggression,which wereusedforthepresentstudy:sexualcontact,sexualcoercion, attemptedrape,andrape.Inaddition,aglobalscoreiscomputed byaddingupthefrequency ofeachitem.Kossand Oros(1982) founda Cronbach’sreliability equal to.79 and good test-retest reliabilityafterone-week.Inthepresentstudy,Cronbach’salpha was.91 fortheaggressionversionand.86 forthevictimization version.
Procedure
Between2011and2012,maleadolescentswererecruited to participate ina study ofsexualattitudes. Anauthorizationand consentformwaspreviouslygiventoparentsandschoolteachers.
Studentswhoagreedtoparticipatecompletedapaperandpencil versionofthequestionnaireingroupsof30orfewerinanavailable classroom.Participantsweresittingsufficientlyfaraparttoensure privacy.Oncetheparticipantscompletedthebooklet,thesurvey wasreturnedtotheexperimenterinasealedenvelope.Anonymity, confidentiality, and freedom to withdraw fromthe study were guaranteed.Estimatedcompletiontimeofthequestionnaireswas 30to45minutes.Participantsreceivednocompensationfortheir participationinthestudy.
Results
For the subsequent statistical analyses, the variable sexual aggressionwasdichotomizedasyes/no.“Yes”wasselectedwhen theparticipantreportedtohaveperpetratedsometypeofsexual aggression(i.e.,sexualcontact,sexualcoercion,attemptedrape, orrape)atleastonceinhis/herlifeandwasassignedthevalue 1, whereas“no” wasselectedwhen theparticipant reportedto havenever perpetratedsexualaggressionand wasassigned the value0.Thesameprocedurewasconductedforthevariable“sexual victimization”.
Table2showsthatperpetratorsreportedthefollowingpreva- lence by subtype of aggression: 24.8% sexual contact (i.e., non-penetrative sexual contact, such as kissing, fondling, etc., with an unwilling partner); 14.3% sexual coercion (i.e., sexual intercourse withoutanindividual’s consentbymeansof verbal pressure oruseofauthority); 12.5%attemptedrape; and10.3%
rape.Regardingthenumberoftimessexualaggressionwasper- petrated,52%oftheparticipantsreportedperpetratingaggressive sexualbehavioronlyonce,whereas48%reportedengaginginsex- ualaggressionmorethanonce.Onaverage,aggressors reported perpetratingsexualaggression3.02times(SD=2.99).Inaddition, thevictim perpetratedby subtypeof aggressionwasindicated, consideringthedistinctionfromtheSES:stranger,acquaintance, datingpartner(casualpartner),andex/currentpartner(involved inastablerelationship).
SexualDoubleStandardandRapeSupportiveAttitudesinSexual AggressorsandNon-Aggressors
T-testsforindependentsampleswereconductedtoexploredif- ferencesbetweensexualaggressorsandnon-aggressorsinsexual doublestandardandattitudestowardsrape.Wefoundsignificant differencesinbothsexualdoublestandard,t(446)=-3.72,p=.000, Cohen’sd=0.18,andattitudetowardsrape,t(446)=-2.64,p=.000, Cohen’sd=0.13. Thatis,compared tothenon-aggressor group, thesexualaggressorgroupreportedasignificantlyhighersexual double standard (non aggressors M=25.87, SD=5.22; aggres- sorsM=27.88,SD=5.69)andmoresupportiveattitudestowards rape (non aggressors M=59.23, SD=9.20; aggressors M=61.66, SD=9.14).
In addition, the Pearson correlation between sexual double standardandrapesupportiveattitudesshowedasignificantasso- ciation(r=.46,p=.000).
PredictorVariablesofSexualAggression
Weconductedalogisticregressionanalysistoexplorethepre- dictorvariablesofsexualaggression.Thefollowingvariableswere enteredaspredictors:adolescentsexualvictimization,sexualdou- blestandard,andsupportiverapeattitudes.Weusedthebackward
Table2
FrequencyandPercentageofEachSubtypeofSexualAggressionandVictimPerpetrated.
Sexualaggression n % Victim n
Sexualcontact Yes 111 24.8 Stranger 6
Acquaintance 11
Datingpartner 34
Ex/curentpartner 10
No 337
Sexualcoercion Yes 64 14.3 Stranger 2
Acquaintance 6
Datingpartner 13
Ex/currentpartner 3
No 384
Attemptedrape Yes 56 12.5 Stranger 2
Acquaintance 7
Datingpartner 9
Ex/currentpartner 4
No 285
Rape Yes 46 10.3 Stranger 3
Acquaintance 2
Datingpartner 10
Ex/currentpartner 1
No 291
Table3
SummaryoftheLogisticRegressionwithSexualAggressionastheDependentVariable.
B SE Wald p Odds 95%CI
Adolescentvictimization 2.35 0.24 94.20 .000 10.48 6.52-16.84
Rapesupportiveattitudes 0.02 0.01 4.51 .034 1.02 1.00-1.05
conditionaleliminationmethodtoreducethenumberofTypeII errorsassociatedwithforwardmethods.Thefinalregressionmodel wassignificant(p<.001,df=2)andcorrectlyclassified67%ofthe sample.Resultsrevealedthattheperpetrationofsexualaggression byadolescentmaleswasbestpredictedbyhavebeensexualvictim ofabuseduringadolescenceandsupportiveattitudestowardsrape (seeTable3).
Discussion
Theaimofthisstudywastoexplorethevariablesthatareasso- ciatedwithandwhichbestpredictself-reportedsexualaggression inasampleofPeruvianadolescentmales.Ourresultsrevealedthat 33.3%ofthesamplereportedhavingperpetratedsexualaggres- sionatleastonce.Ofthetotalsample,accordingtothesubtype ofsexual aggression,prevalencerangedfrom 10.3%for rapeto 24.8%forsexualcontactwithanunwillingpartner.Themostfre- quentvictimofanytypeofsexualaggressionwasadatingpartner (casualpartner),whereastheleastfrequentwasastrangeroran ex/currentpartner(forrape).Theseresultsarecomparabletoother prevalencerates of victimizationforsamplesin Spain(Moyano
&Sierra,2015;Santos-Iglesias&Sierra,2012;Vega-Gea,Ortega- Ruiz,&Sánchez,2016),theUnitedStates(Breidingetal.,2015), andvariousLatinAmericancountriessuchasChile(Lehrer,Lehrer, Lehrer,&Oyarzún,2007),Mexico(Cortés-Ayalaetal.,2014),and Brazil(D’Abreu,Krahé,&Bazon,2013).Ourfindingsindicatehigh prevalencerates,takingintoaccountthatparticipants–whoseages rangedfrom14to17–were askedaboutaggressionscommitted sincetheageof14.
Whencomparing sexual aggressors and non-aggressors, we foundthatsexualaggressorsreportedhavingfeweregalitariangen- derbeliefsandmoresupportiveattitudestowardsrape.However, Cohen’sdwasnotveryrobust.Moreover,sexualdoublestandard wasfoundtobeassociatedwithrapesupportiveattitudes(r=46).
Thisisconsistentwithpreviousstudiesinwhichtheendorsement
oftraditional genderroleswasassociated withthejustification offorcefulactions(Shenetal.,2012;Sierra,Mongeetal.,2010;
Sierra,Santos-Iglesias,Gutiérrez-Quintanilla,Bermúdez,&Buela- Casal,2010).
Inthelogisticregressionanalysis,datarevealedthatthebest predictorsof sexualaggressionwereself-reportedabuseduring adolescenceandrapesupportiveattitudes.Thiswasthecasefor 67%ofthesample.Thesefindingssupport,ontheonehandthe cycleofsexualcoercion(Ward&Beech,2006),thatis,individuals whohavebeenvictimizedaremorelikelytobecomeaggressors.
Ontheotherhand,maleswhotendtoperpetratesuchviolenceare morelikelytoacceptjustificationsforrape.Aspreviouslyshown by Maxwell,Robinson, and Post (2002), attitudes towards vio- lencepredictedmaledatingaggressioninasampleofadolescents.
Consistently,Sierra etal.(2009)concludedthatrapesupportive attitudespredictedsexualcoercioninasampleofuniversitystu- dentsfromSalvador.Thus,violencemightbejustifiedbyblaming thevictim. Therefore,justificationofviolenceisarelevantvari- ableforaggression(Ruiz-Hernández,García-Jiménez,Llor-Esteban,
&Godoy-Fernández,2015),whichhasoftenbeenassociatedwith hostilesexism(Lila,Oliver,Catalá-Mi ˜nana,Galiana,&Gracia,2014).
Previousresearchhasrevealedanassociationbetweenbeinga sexualvictimandlaterperpetratingsexualcoercioninmen(e.g., Moyano&Sierra,2015;Russell&Oswald,2002).Prevalenceofvic- timizationinPeruisamongthehighestfromLatinAmerica(61%) (Garcia-Morenoetal.,2006).Inaddition,40%ofPeruvianadoles- centsandyoungstersinitiatetheirsexlifebyforcedintercourse (Cáceres,Marin,&Hudes,2000).Althoughinthepresentstudywe didnotanalyzewhoweretheperpetratorsofboys’victimization, datafromtheNationalActionPlanforChildhoodandAdolescence (2012-2021)fromPeruindicatethatsexualabuseattheseages tendtobeperpetratedbyaclosely-relatedindividual.Inthissense, theuseofparents’violenceinLatinAmericaishigh,beingboys thosemorehighlyaffected(Gage&Silvestre,2010).Otherdata, fromGuatemalaandSalvador,indicatethat46%and42%ofmen
respectively,reportedbeingbeatenaspunishmentatyoungerages (Speizer,Goodwin,Samandari,Kim,&Clyde,2008).
Inourstudy,themostreportedvictimperpetratedwasadat- ingpartner(casualpartner).Itislikelythattheseparticipantsare frequentlyinvolvedinviolentrelationshipsinwhichtheyarevic- timsandaggressorsatthesametime.Althoughpreviousresearch showthataggressionismorefrequentlypresentedincommitted datingrelationships(e.g.,Hanley&O’Neill,1997),researchwith collegestudents(Klipfel,Claxton,&vanDulmen,2014)andwith adolescentssuggeststheco-occurrenceofsexualrisk-taking,such as havingcasualpartners and sexual dating aggression(Gover, Kaukinen,&Fox,2008).
Contrarytoexpectations,andincontrastwiththestudycon- ducted by Sierra et al.(2009), sexualdouble standard wasnot foundtoberelevanttothepredictionof sexualaggression.Our findingsratherindicatethatattitudesthatsupportandjustifycer- tainaggressivebehaviorsaremoreimportantthanbeliefsonthe dominanceof malesover females,at least in adolescence.This is consistentwith a review of thedynamic risk factors of dat- ingviolencefromcontinentalEurope,andfromtheUnitedStates andCanada(Leenetal.,2013).Theauthorssuggestthatattitudes towardsviolence,suchastheacceptanceofrapemyths,tolerance ofviolence,andjustificationoftheuseofviolence,werereported asriskfactorsfordatingviolenceperpetration.
Theendorsementofsexualdoublestandardattitudesinitself maynotbesufficienttoperpetrateaggression.Asitiswellknown, sexualdoublestandardentailsthedominanceofonegroup(men) over another (women), which is aligned withthe construct of social dominanceorientation,definedas thedegreetowhich a personholdsanti-egalitarianvaluesandapreferenceforgroup- basedhierarchies(Sidanius&Pratto,2004).Similartosexualdouble standard,mentendtoexhibitmoresocialdominanceorientation.
Inastudyofadolescents,Mayeux(2014)foundthatsocialdom- inanceorientationdidnot predictaggression. Anotherstudyof Chineseadolescentsshowedthatthestrongestpredictorofsex- ualaggression wasthe boys’attitudes towardsdating violence ratherthantheirtraditionalgenderbeliefs(Shenetal.,2012).Taken together,sexualdoublestandardmightbemorerelevant(atleast duringadolescence)tothepredictionofprejudiceattitudes,butnot totheperpetrationofviolence.
Inasimilarvein,a reviewbyFloodandPease(2009)onthe factorsthatinfluenceattitudestowardsviolenceagainstwomen, suggestedthatgeneralgenderattitudescannotbetakenasasimple proxyforattitudestoviolenceagainstwomen.Asemphasizedby Murnen,Wright,andKaluzny(2002),measuresofgeneralgender- roleattitudeshavelesspowertopredictmalesexualaggression thanothermeasuresofpatriarchalmasculinebeliefsinparticular.
Anotherlineofinterpretationsregardingnon-significantasso- ciation of non-egalitarian beliefs with violence is that male’s perceptionsoftheattitudesofthefemalepartnermaybeimpor- tantwhenconsideringtheuseofviolenceasawayforthemale to recover dominance. More specifically, some women might endorsesimilar non-egalitarian attitudes,and thus not provide anyresistancetomen’sattitudes.Basedontheconfluencemodel (Malamuth, 2003), oneof themostcomprehensive explanatory modelsofmalesexualaggression,gratificationfromdominating women is a predictor of sexualaggression (within the“hostile masculinity”factor).Inotherwords,malesexualaggressionmay bemotivatedbydominanceandpower,aspreviouslysuggested (Russell & Oswald, 2001; Zurbriggen, 2000). In a similar vein, malesexualattitudesareoftennormalized.Consequently,there isalsoacertainlevelofconstantpressureamongboystobehavein sexuallyaggressiveways,andgirlsaresometimeslikelytoaccom- modatemaleneedsanddesiresinnegotiatingtheirsexualrelations (Hird&Jackson,2001;Tolman,Spencer,Rosen-Reynoso,&Porche, 2003).
Thisstudyobtainedself-reporteddatafromanadolescentsam- pleofPeruvianboys.Todate,littleresearchhasapproachedthe studyofboth gender-attitudesand rape-attitudesinadolescent samples.Therefore,this studyprovidesvaluabledatafor young boysinthisagegroup.Overall,wefoundthatkeyfactorsinthe predictionofsexualaggressionwere:(1)havingbeenavictimof sexualaggressionand(2)theendorsementofsupportiveattitudes towardsrape.Preventionprogramswithaggressorsandpsychoso- cialinterventionsshouldaddressthesefactors,inparticular,those relatedtoviolence-supportiveattitudesandtothejustificationof violence.Thisisimportantbecauseuntilnowmostinterventions havemainlyfocusedongenderinequality,whichisoftenseenas thesoleexplanatoryfactorforgenderviolence(Carbajosa&Boira, 2013).Therefore,thereisaspecialneedformultimodalinterven- tions(seeArce&Fari ˜na,2010),whichintegrateattitudestowards violenceanditsjustification.
Beyondinterventions,thesefindingsshouldbeintegratedinto legalandpoliticalstrategiesproposedbythePeruviangovernment.
Forthelastdecade,severallegalproceedingshavebeendeveloped forthepreventionofsexualabuseinchildrenandadolescentsand thepromotionofgenderequalityinPeru.Someofthesepropo- salsarematerializedintheNationalActionPlanforChildhoodand Adolescence(2012-2021)andtheNationalPlanforGenderEquality (2012-2017).Bothprogramsestablishstrategieswiththepriority toreducesexualviolenceamongchildrenandadolescents,through thepromotionofresearchabouttheriskfactorsofsexualabuseand educationcampaignsbasedonagenderequalityapproach.
This study had certain limitations. First at all, the adoles- cent sample covered a narrow age range (from 14 to 17). A broaderpicturewould havebeenobtainedifyoungerboyshad alsobeenincorporated.Secondly, giventhatthis researchhasa cross-sectional,correlationaldesign,nocausaleffectscanbeestab- lished.Inthissense,alongitudinalstudywouldhaveallowedusto bettertrackindividualsduringtheiradolescenceandevenonto theiradulttransition.Thiswouldhaveundoubtedlyenrichedthe understandingofsexualaggression.Thirdly,itislikelythatsexual doublestandardmaynotonlyberelatedtosexualaggression,but alsotopsychologicalorverbalaggression(i.e.,disagreementswith thepartneraboutroleexpectations).Fourthly,consideringthatall informationobtainedinthisstudywasbasedonself-reports,social desirabilityshouldhavebeenmeasuredandcontrolled.“Macho”
scriptsarestillrobustinPeru,thusmalesmaybeexaggeratingor minimizingtheirreportoftheiraggressiveness.Finally,regarding victimizationinformation,moredataaboutthecharacteristicsof theperpetratorofthosewhoreporttohavecommittedaggression, shouldbeofinterest(beingvictimizedbyadultsversuspeers).
Theaforementionedmeansthatothermeasurementsofsex- ualaggression,whichencompassabroaderspectrumofviolence and perpetrator’s characteristics, should beconsidered for fur- therresearch.Finally,futurestudiesshouldconsidereithermale’s perceptionofhispartner’sendorsementofthesamesexualstan- dardsorbeliefs(asaggressionmostlikelystemsfromdisagreement between both perspectives) or data collection from the part- ner.Thistypeofdyadicanalysiswouldprovideknowledgeabout whetherbothpartnerssharethesamegenderbeliefs.
Nonetheless,theresultsofthis studyprovidevaluableinfor- mation for education and intervention programs with male adolescents,whoareavulnerabletargetforpreventionpurposes.
Educationalprogramsshouldpayspecialattentiontoandfocuson theawarenessofrapesupportiveattitudes.
ConflictofInterest
Theauthorsofthisarticledeclarenoconflictofinterest.
Acknowledgments
ThisworkwasfundedbytheAgenciaEspa ˜noladeCooperación InternacionalparaelDesarrollo(ResearchProjectA/030018/10).
References
Arce,R.,&Fari ˜na,F.(2010).Dise ˜noeimplementacióndelProgramaGaliciade ReeducacióndeMaltratadores:Unarespuestapsicosocialaunanecesidadsocial ypenitenciaria.IntervenciónPsicosocial,19,153–166.
Breiding,M.J.,Smith,S.G.,Basile,K.C.,Walters,M.L.,Chen,J.,&Merrick,M.T.
(2015).Prevalenceandcharacteristicsofsexualviolence,stalking,andintimate partnerviolencevictimization—NationalIntimatePartnerandSexualViolence Survey,UnitedStates,2011.AmericanJournalofPublicHealth,105,E11.
Byers,E.S.(1996).Howwelldoesthetraditionalsexualscriptexplainsexualcoer- cion?Reviewofaprogramofresearch.JournalofPsychology&HumanSexuality, 8,7–25.
Cáceres,C.(2005).Assessingyoungpeople’snon-consensualsexualexperiences:
LessonsfromPeru.InS.Jejeebhoy,I.Shah,&S.Tapa(Eds.),SexwithoutConsent:
YoungpeopleinDevelopingCountries(pp.127–138).London:ZedBooks.
Cáceres,C.F.,Marin,B.V.,&Hudes,E.S.(2000).Sexualcoercionamongyouthand youngadultsinLima,Peru.JournalofAdolescentHealth,27,361–367.
Carbajosa,P.,&Boira,S.(2013).Estadoactualyretosfuturosdelosprogramaspara hombrescondenadosporviolenciadegéneroenEspa ˜na.PsychosocialInterven- tion,22,145–152.http://dx.doi.org/10.5093/in2013a17
Caron,S.L.,Davis,C.M.,Halteman,W.A.,&Stickle,M.(1993).Predictorsofcondom relatedbehavioursamongfirstyearcollegestudents.JournalofSexResearch,30, 252–259.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499309551709
Cortés-Ayala,M.L.,BringasMolleda,C.,Rodríguez-Franco, L.,Flores Galaz,M., Ramiro-Sánchez,T.,&RodríguezDíaz,F.J.(2014).Unperceiveddatingviolence amongMexicanstudents.InternationalJournalofClinicalandHealthPsychology, 14,39–47.
D’Abreu, L.C. F., Krahé, B.,&Bazon, M.R. (2013).Sexual aggression among Braziliancollegestudents:Prevalenceofvictimizationandperpetrationinmen andwomen.JournalofSexResearch,50,795–807.
DeGue,S.,DeLillo,D.,&Scalora,M.(2010).Areallperpetratorsalike?Comparingrisk factorsforsexualcoercionandaggression.SexualAbuse:AJournalofResearchand Treatment,22,402–426.
Emmers-Sommer,T.M.,Farrell,J.,Gentry,A.,Stevens,S.,Eckstein,J.,Battocletti,J.,
&Gardener,C.(2010).Firstdatesexualexpectations:Theeffectsofwhoasked, whopaid,datelocation,andgender.CommunicationStudies,61,339–355.
Fasula,A.M.,Carry,M.,&Miller,K.S.(2014).Amultidimensionalframeworkforthe meaningsofthesexualdoublestandardanditsapplicationforthesexualhealth ofyoungblackwomenintheUS.TheJournalofSexResearch,51,170–183.
Fitzpatrick,M.K.,Salgado,D.M.,Suvak,M.K.,King,L.A.,&King,D.W.(2004).
Associationsofgenderandgender-roleideologywithbehavioralandattitudi- nalfeaturesofintimatepartneraggression.PsychologyofMen&Masculinity,5, 91–102.
Flood,M.,&Pease,B.(2009).Factorsinfluencingattitudestoviolenceagainst women.Trauma,Violence,&Abuse,10,125–142.
Gage,A.J.,&Silvestre,E.A.(2010).Maternalviolence,victimization,andchild physicalpunishmentinPeru.ChildAbuse&Neglect,34,523–533.
Gallagher,K.E.,&Parrott, D.J. (2011).Whataccounts formen’shostileatti- tudestoward women? Theinfluence ofhegemonicmalerole norms and masculinegender role stress. Violence AgainstWomen, 17, 568–583, doi:
1077801211407296.
Gannon,T.A.,Rose,M.R.,&Ward,T.(2008).Adescriptivemodeloftheoffense processforfemalesexualoffenders.SexualAbuse:AJournalofResearchand Treatment,20,352–374.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1079063208322495 Garcia-Moreno,C.,Jansen,H.A.,Ellsberg,M.,Heise,L.,&Watts,C.H.(2006).Preva-
lenceofintimatepartnerviolence:findingsfromtheWHOmulti-countrystudy onwomen’shealthanddomesticviolence.TheLancet,368,1260–1269.
Gomez,A.M.,Speizer,I.S.,&Moracco,K.E.(2011).Linkagesbetweengenderequity andintimatepartnerviolenceamongurbanBrazilianyouth.JournalofAdolescent Health,49,393–399.
Gover,A.R.,Kaukinen,C.,&Fox,K.A.(2008).Therelationshipbetweenviolence inthefamilyoforiginanddatingviolenceamongcollegestudents.Journalof InterpersonalViolence,23,1667–1693.
Greene,K.,&Faulkner,S.L.(2005).Gender,beliefinthesexualdoublestandard,and sexualtalkinheterosexualdatingrelationships.SexRoles,53,239–251.
Güezmes,A.,&Vargas,L.(2003).Proyectoparacombatirlaviolenciabasadaen géneroenAméricaLatina.InformefinaldelacomparaciónentreLineaBasaly LineadeSalidaenINPPARES,PLAFAMyPROFAMILIA.PLAFAMyPROFAMILIA.
NewYork:InternationalPlannedParenthoodFederation/WesternHemisphere Region(IPPF/WHR).
Gutiérrez-Quintanilla,J.R.,Rojas-García,A.,&Sierra,J.C.(2010).Cross-culturalcom- parisonofthesexualdoublestandardbetweenSalvadoranandSpanishcollege students.SalvadoranJournalofPsychology,1,31–51.
Hanley,M.J.,&O’Neill,P.(1997).Violenceandcommitmentastudyofdatingcouples.
JournalofInterpersonalViolence,12,685–703.
Helmus,L.,Hanson,R.K.,Thornton,D.,Babchishin,K.M.,&Harris,A.J.(2012).
Absoluterecidivismratespredictedbystatic-99randstatic-2002Rsexoffender riskassessmenttoolsvaryacrosssamples:Ameta-analysis.CriminalJusticeand Behavior,39,1148–1171.
Hird,M.J.,&Jackson,S.(2001).Where‘angels’and‘wusses’feartotread:Sexual coercioninadolescentdatingrelationships.JournalofSociology,37,27–43.
Jewkes,R.,&Sikweyiya,Y.(2013).Streamlining:UnderstandinggangrapeinSouth Africa.InM.Horvath,&J.Woodhams(Eds.),Handbookonthestudyofmul- tipleperpetratorrape:Amultidisciplinaryresponsetoaninternationalproblem (pp.116–131).London:Routledge.
Johnson,B.E.,Kuck,D.L.,&Schander,P.R.(1997).Rapemythacceptanceand sociodemographiccharacteristics:Amultidimensionalanalysis.SexRoles,36, 693–707.
Klipfel,K.M.,Claxton,S.E.,&vanDulmen,M.H.(2014).Interpersonalaggres- sionvictimizationwithincasualsexualrelationshipsandexperiences.Journal ofInterpersonalViolence,29,557–569.
Koss,M.P.,Abbey,A.,Campbell,R.,Cook,S.,Norris,J.,Testa,M.,...,&White,J.
(2007).RevisingtheSES:Acollaborativeprocesstoimproveassessmentof sexual aggression and victimization. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 357–370.
Koss,M.P.,&Oros,C.J.(1982).SexualExperiencesSurvey:Aresearchinstrument investigatingsexualaggressionandvictimization.JournalofConsultingandClin- icalPsychology,50,455–457.
Kreager,D.A.,&Staff,J.(2009).Thesexualdoublestandardandadolescentpeer acceptance.SocialPsychologyQuarterly,72,143–164.
Leen,E.,Sorbring,E.,Mawer,M.,Holdsworth,E.,Helsing,B.,&Bowen,E.(2013).
Prevalence,dynamicriskfactorsandtheefficacyofprimaryinterventionsfor adolescentdatingviolence:Aninternationalreview.AggressionandViolent Behavior,18,159–174.
Lees,S.(2002).Carnalknowledge:Rapeontrail.London:TheWomen’sPressLtd.
Lehrer,J.A.,Lehrer,V.L.,Lehrer,E.L.,&Oyarzún,P.B.(2007).Prevalenceofandrisk factorsforsexualvictimizationincollegewomeninChile.InternationalFamily PlanningPerspectives,33,168–175.
Lila, M., Oliver, A., Catalá-Mi ˜nana, A., Galiana, L., & Gracia, E. (2014).
The Intimate Partner Violence Responsibility Attribution Scale (IPVRAS).
The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 6, 29–36.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5093/ejpalc2014a4
Lonsway,K.A.,&Fitzgerald,L.F.(1994).Rapemythsinreview.PsychologyofWomen Quarterly,18,133–164.
Lottes,I.L.(1991).Beliefsystems:Sexualityandrape.JournalofPsychologyand HumanSexuality,4,37–59.
Lyons,H.,Giordano,P.C.,Manning,W.D.,&Longmore,M.A.(2011).Identity,peer relationships,andadolescentgirls’sexualbehavior:Anexplorationofthecon- temporarydoublestandard.JournalofSexResearch,48,437–449.
Malamuth, N. M. (2003). Criminal and noncriminal sexual aggres- sors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 989, 33–58.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07292.x
Manago,A.M.,Greenfield,P.M.,Kim,J.L.,&Ward,L.M.(2014).Changingcultural pathwaysthroughgenderroleandsexualdevelopment:Atheoreticalframe- work.Ethos,42,198–221.
Marín,B.V.,Gómez,C.A.,Tschann,J.M.,&Gregorich,S.E.(1997).Condomuse inunmarriedLatinomen:Atestofculturalconstructs.HealthPsychology,16, 458–467.
Marston,C.,&King,E.(2006).Factorsthatshapeyoungpeople’ssexualbehaviour:
Asystematicreview.TheLancet,368,1581–1586.
Maxwell,C.D.,Robinson,A.J.,&Post,L.A.(2002).Thenatureandpredictorsofsexual victimisationandoffendingamongadolescents.JournalofYouthandAdolescence, 32,465–477.
Mayeux,L.(2014).Understandingpopularityandrelationalaggressioninado- lescence:Theroleofsocialdominanceorientation.SocialDevelopment,23, 502–517.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sode.12054
Milhausen, R. R.,&Herold,E.S.(2002).Reconceptualizing thesexual double standard.JournalofPsychology&HumanSexuality,13,63–83.
Ministerio de la Mujer y Poblaciones Vulnerables, MIMP (2015). Plan anual de monitoreo y evaluación de las intervenciones del PNCVFS reporte abreviado de seguimiento del mes de junio 2015. Retrieved June 7, 2016 fromhttp://www.mimp.gob.pe/files/PAME/PAME-Reporte-de-Junio-2015.pdf Monge,F.S.,Sierra,J.C.,&Salinas,J.M.(2013).Equivalenciafactorialymétricade
laDoubleStandardScaleporsexoyedad.SumaPsicológica,20,7–14.
Moyano, N., & Sierra, J. C. (2015). Sexual victimisation, sexual cogni- tions, desire and excitation/inhibition in community Spanish male and female sexual aggressors. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 22, 36–51.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13552600.2014.996614
Murnen,S.K.,Wright,C.,&Kaluzny,G.(2002).Ifboyswillbeboys,thengirlswillbe victims?Ameta-analyticreviewoftheresearchthatrelatesmasculineideology tosexualaggression.SexRoles,46,359–375.
Povedano,A.,Cava,M.J.,Monreal,M.C.,Varela,R.,&Musitu,G.(2015).Victim- ization,loneliness,overtandrelationalviolenceatthesocialfromagender perspective.InternationalJournalofClinicalandHealthPsychology,15,44–51.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2014.09.001
Raffaelli,M.,&Ontai,L.L.(2001).‘She’s16yearsoldandthere’sboyscallingover tothehouse’:AnexploratorystudyofsexualsocializationinLatinofamilies.
Culture,Health&Sexuality,3,295–310.
Ruiz-Hernández,J.A.,García-Jiménez,J.J.,Llor-Esteban,B.,&Godoy-Fernández, C. (2015). Risk factors for intimate partner violence in prison inmates.
The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 7, 41–49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpal.2014.11.003
Russell,B.L.,&Oswald,D.L.(2001).Strategiesanddispositionalcorrelatesofsexual coercionperpetratedbywomen:Anexploratoryinvestigation.SexRoles,45, 103–115.http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1013016502745